Featured Linguist!

Jost Gippert: Our Featured Linguist!

"Buenos dias", "buenas noches" -- this was the first words in a foreign language I heard in my life, as a three-year old boy growing up in developing post-war Western Germany, where the first gastarbeiters had arrived from Spain. Fascinated by the strange sounds, I tried to get to know some more languages, the only opportunity being TV courses of English and French -- there was no foreign language education for pre-teen school children in Germany yet in those days. Read more



Donate Now | Visit the Fund Drive Homepage

Amount Raised:

$34168

Still Needed:

$40832

Can anyone overtake Syntax in the Subfield Challenge ?

Grad School Challenge Leader: University of Washington


Publishing Partner: Cambridge University Press CUP Extra Publisher Login
amazon logo
More Info


New from Oxford University Press!

ad

What is English? And Why Should We Care?

By: Tim William Machan

To find some answers Tim Machan explores the language's present and past, and looks ahead to its futures among the one and a half billion people who speak it. His search is fascinating and important, for definitions of English have influenced education and law in many countries and helped shape the identities of those who live in them.


New from Cambridge University Press!

ad

Medical Writing in Early Modern English

Edited by Irma Taavitsainen and Paivi Pahta

This volume provides a new perspective on the evolution of the special language of medicine, based on the electronic corpus of Early Modern English Medical Texts, containing over two million words of medical writing from 1500 to 1700.


Summary Details


Query:   Development of Agreement Morphology - part 1
Author:  Eric Fuss
Submitter Email:  click here to access email
Linguistic LingField(s):   Morphology
Syntax

Summary:   Some weeks ago I posted a query (LINGUIST 13.2217) asking for
information on languages which currently show a development of subject
agreement morphology from pronouns (or have undergone such a change in
their recent history).

I want to thank all who responded for their very helpful and in many
cases detailed messages, which provided me not only with a lot of
interesting data but also helped me to gain new insights in my topic
(the development of agreement morphology and its syntactic
consequences). Thanks a lot!

Several people (Werner Abraham, Stefan Ploch, and Helmut Weiss)
referred me to the well-known phenomenon of complementizer agreement
which is found in a variety of southern German dialects. Here, the
relevant agreement morphology developed out of enclitic subject
pronouns which attach to the second clausal position (occupied by the
finite verb in main clauses and the complementizer in subordinate
clauses). The fact that the inflected complementizer co-occurs with
another subject shows that the former pronoun has been reanalyzed as
an agreement marker, thereby losing its argument
status. Interestingly, in some dialects, weak pronouns of this kind
also developed into a new marker of verbal agreement, which can be
seen from the fact that the relevant morphology spread to finite verbs
in clause final position as well (only 1st & 2nd ps):

(1)wa'ma doch zwou kei kod hama
because-2pl particle two cows had have-2sg
'...because we had two cows'

Wolfgang Schulze turned my attention to the East Caucasian languages,
where very interesting developments are underway. The languages spoken
in the Eastern Caucasus are often characterized by a system of noun
classes which are marked on the verb in an Erg/Abs fashion. Apart from
this kind of (ergative) noun class agreement, however, many languages
(e.g. Lak, Tabasaran, Awar, Dargwa) have developed (and still develop)
an additional agreement paradigm of person (and sometimes number)
marking on the verb. Interestingly, this new agreement paradigm is
often Nom/Acc oriented. The degree of grammaticalization of this new
paradigm varies greatly from language to language. The first
distinction that arises is apparently first person vs. non-first
person agent. Finer-grained systems referring to other person/number
distinctions may develop subsequently.

Bill Morris took considerable time to give me some detailed
information on Kapampangan (Northern Philippines) which has undergone
a shift from pronouns to obligatory ergative and absolutive
agreement. The resulting agreement cluster is usually in second
position right after the verb, but it can appear in front of the verb
in some contexts. Furthermore, the agreement cluster can be divided
into ergative and absolutive agreement clitics by a particle meaning
'just now'. These facts suggest that the agreement cluster has not yet
developed into a verbal affix.

Mark Donohue reported that Skou (North-Central New Guinea) is
currently developing a second cycle of agreement markers. Skou
exhibits a bewildering number of different strategies to mark
agreement with a subject (pronominal agreement clitics, vowel and
consonant alternations on the verb, as well as combinations of
these). In a recent paper, Donohue shows that the kind of multiple
exponence/agreement found in Skou can be attributed to repeated
cliticization of pronominal subjects in the history of this language:
the consonant alternations on the verb are apparently the result of an
earlier process in which subject pronouns fused to the verb. The
subsequent reduction of this kind of distinctive agreement morphology
is in turn compensated by a second wave of cliticization in the
present day language.

To be continued in Part 2.

LL Issue: 13.2514
Date Posted: 02-Oct-2002
Original Query: Read original query


Back

Sums main page