Publishing Partner: Cambridge University Press CUP Extra Publisher Login
amazon logo
More Info


New from Oxford University Press!

ad

Oxford Handbook of Corpus Phonology

Edited by Jacques Durand, Ulrike Gut, and Gjert Kristoffersen

Offers the first detailed examination of corpus phonology and serves as a practical guide for researchers interested in compiling or using phonological corpora


New from Cambridge University Press!

ad

The Languages of the Jews: A Sociolinguistic History

By Bernard Spolsky

A vivid commentary on Jewish survival and Jewish speech communities that will be enjoyed by the general reader, and is essential reading for students and researchers interested in the study of Middle Eastern languages, Jewish studies, and sociolinguistics.


New from Brill!

ad

Indo-European Linguistics

New Open Access journal on Indo-European Linguistics is now available!


Summary Details


Query:   Verbal and Nonverbal Word Orders, Part 2
Author:  Mark Donohue
Submitter Email:  click here to access email
Linguistic LingField(s):   Syntax

Summary:   Regarding query: http://www.linguistlist.org/issues/17/17-411.html#1

Regarding sum: http://www.linguistlist.org/issues/17/17-615.html#1

A few weeks ago I posted a query about the order of subjects and predicates
in nonverbal clauses, such as that seen in (1), from Indonesian.

(1) Dia guru
3sg teacher
‘S/he’s [a] teacher.’

Crucially, I was interested in cases in which the order of the subject and
predicate in the nonverbal clause was different to the order of the subject
and predicate in a verbal clause. In Indonesian this is not the case; but
some languages have, for instance, V-initial order in verbal clauses, but
S-initial order in non-verbal clauses.
(And, of course, many languages have variability in the order of elements,
depending on some semantic property or other. More on this later.)

And I got some responses, and posted a summary that included them, as well
as a clarification of the original question.
All well and good; but since the sum, I’ve received more information from
LL readers, enough to think I should post a follow-up sum. (Advice to
future LL query-posters: wait more than just a couple of weeks to post your
summary!)

We now, thanks to Dick Hudson, Philip Davies, and Bruno Estigarribia, have
some more information. And it’s interesting, and a
confirm-a-hypothesis-that-wasn’t-there-before way.

Bruno points out that (verb-initial) Mixtec or Zapotec show similar
behaviour; and Dick mentions Arabic as a rigidly VSO language that
nonetheless has S PRED order in nonverbal clauses.

(in the previous sum another language that featured strongly was Ge’ez, a
classical language from Ethiopia, which is VSO. Verb-initiality is important?)

Philip mentions Palauan as a language that shows a lot of SVO, but is
underlyingly VOS, yet has S PRED in nonverbal clauses. On its own this is
just another example; but combined with the fact that Enggano and Tukang
Besi also show the same pattern, and that these languages are not only all
related to each other (they’re all Austronesian), but are all found on the
very fringe of the Western Malayo-Polynesian zone (Palauan: far east;
Enggano: south-west; Tukang Besi: far south-east), we have an interesting
distribution.
But Palauan, Enggano and Tukang Besi are widely separated; there’s no
question of mutual interference here. How is it that they all ended up with
the same (typologically rare) pattern?

Now, some more relevant information: Givón (1977), and Payne (1995)
point out that, when you have VS and SV as alternatives, the SV option is
used for more stative, less eventive predicates: and, surely, that will
very nicely characterise the semantics of nonverbal predicates, almost by
(a) definition.

This suggests a possible historical scenario for the Austronesian languages
mentioned earlier: predicate-initial languages have a subject-initial
option, used (among other reasons) when you have less eventive predicates.
This becomes grammaticalised first with nonverbal predicates, and later
spreads to be a general subject-first parameter (the languages in the
‘inside’ of the southern half of the WMP zone are usually SVO, S PRED).

Outside the Austronesian cases, we have V-initial languages with an S PRED
order in nonverbal clauses; no cases of V-final languages with PRED S in
nonverbal clauses. Partly this reflects the lack of motivation for
postposing an S (though, if my speech is representative, the number of
postposed putative ‘antitopics’ has been growing in the last couple of
decades in English: things like ‘It’s hard, that homework.’; and see the
note on Sanskrit in the earlier sum).
But the fact that, if anything, a smaller proportion of V-initial languages
lack copular verbs than for V-medial and V-final languages means that
there’s a definite pattern emerging here.

Thanks again to all who wrote in.

-Mark

References:
Givón, Talmy. 1977. The Drift from VSO to SVO in Biblical Hebrew:
The Pragmatics of Tense-Aspect . Charles Li, ed., Mechanisms of Syntactic
Change: 181-254. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Payne, Doris. 1995. Verb initial languages and information order. In Pamela
Downing and Michael Nooning, eds., Word order in discourse: 449-485.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

LL Issue: 17.771
Date Posted: 14-Mar-2006


Back

Sums main page