LINGUIST List 12.1988

Wed Aug 8 2001

Review: Senft, Nominal Classification (2nd rev.)

Editor for this issue: Terence Langendoen <terrylinguistlist.org>


What follows is another discussion note contributed to our Book Discussion Forum. We expect these discussions to be informal and interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially invited to join in.

If you are interested in leading a book discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available for discussion." (This means that the publisher has sent us a review copy.) Then contact Simin Karimi at siminlinguistlist.org or Terry Langendoen at terrylinguistlist.org.


Directory

  • Arvi Hurskainen, review of Gunter Senft (ed.), Systems of Nominal Classification

    Message 1: review of Gunter Senft (ed.), Systems of Nominal Classification

    Date: Fri, 3 Aug 2001 18:12:33 +0300 (EEST)
    From: Arvi Hurskainen <ahurskailing.helsinki.fi>
    Subject: review of Gunter Senft (ed.), Systems of Nominal Classification


    Senft, Gunter, ed. (2000) Systems of Nominal Classification. Cambridge University Press, ISBN 0-521-77075-0, vii+350pp, Language, culture and cognition 4.

    Previous review at http://linguistlist.org/issues/12/12-645.html#1

    Arvi Hurskainen, Institute for Asian and African Studies, University of Helsinki.

    Although the problem of nominal classification has been a subject of discussion and research at least since the appearance of Die nominalen Klassifikations-Systeme in den Sprachen der Erde (Royen 1929), the subject continues to remain fragmented and we still are far from satisfactory treatment of this vast and complicated field. Recent attempts to illuminate the subject include Apprehension (3 vols.) by the Cologne group (Seiler and Lehmann 1982; Seiler and Stachowiak 1982; Seiler 1986), Noun Classes and Categorization (Craig 1986), and the more recent Gender in Grammar and Cognition (Unterbeck and Rissanen, eds.), which the book to be reviewed does not recognise. Because the book tries to cover all types of languages as well as all variations of nominal classification, without consciously excluding any area or type, the task is formidable. It would be easy to point out issues that are either left out or that are not treated satisfactorily, but this is necessarily the case with a book such as this. Therefore, it is more important to see how the authors have succeeded in what they intended to accomplish. Although the geographical field is global, the theoretical viewpoint is defined more narrowly 'to clarify the interface between anthropological and grammatical work on nominal classification'. Whether this approach has made the work more manageable is another issue, since the combination of two distinct research fields, each with established concepts and prejudices towards the other, does not make the work easy. Although most of the chapters have what is vaguely called 'an anthropological linguistic perspective', the contributions are independent articles rather than integrated chapters of a single book. This notion should not be taken as a critic, however, because detail and faithfulness to data should not be sacrificed for an integrated theory. In fact, although the book has an ambitious title, it is the individual contributions, some of which are excellent and very detailed, that are most valuable in it. The research team of the Department of Language and Cognition at the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics in Nijmegen had a coordinating role in compiling this book, which is partly based on articles presented in the workshop entitled 'Back to Basic Issues in Nominal Classification', held in Nijmegen in 1993. More specifically, the book addresses such questions as: What are the basic parameters, and/or the basic semantic distinctions that are expressed in nominal classification? What is actually classified - extra-linguistic referents or the kinds of nouns within the language? What functions do the various types of nominal classification fulfil and how do they function in discourse? To what extent do they presuppose obligatory distinctions and to what extent do they allow freedom in allocating category? The overall fundamental question is: How is the perceived world expressed in, and through, various systems of nominal classification that are grammatically encoded in various languages? The first chapter by Gunter Senft outlines the basic questions to be dealt with in various chapters of the book. Drawing data from Kilivila, an Austronesian language of the Trobriand Islanders, he describes the complex use of the so called classificatory particles in this language and suggests that the issues relevant in the so called 'classifier languages', which Kilivila is a member of, are more general and are relevant also for other systems of nominal classification. Senft lists and describes briefly various systems of nominal classification found among languages. Such systems include: (a) languages that classify the nouns according to kind and degree of possession (Kilivila being an example of such a system); (b) languages that utilise classificatory noun incorporation, where a generic noun is syntactically incorporated into the verb and cross-classifies a specific noun which is syntactically governed by the verb; (c) classification by verb, where verb roots provide a semantically transparent classification of the intransitive subject or transitive object; (d) the so-called 'classifier languages' with numeral classification, where, when counting inanimate as well as animate referents, the numerals concatenate with a certain morpheme, which is the so-called 'classifier'; (e) noun class systems of nominal classification with a fairly high degree of grammaticalization, a closed system of classes and a limited degree of semantic transparency (e.g. Bantu languages); and (f) gender systems where the determining criterion is agreement. As Senft points out, this list is not exhaustive, and there is overlap between categories. Especially the terms 'noun class system' and 'gender system' are not clear, and some researchers (e.g. Corbett) consider noun classes as part of the wider gender system. Finally there are languages that apply the classifier system and the noun class system simultaneously, which gives reason to suspect that a language may be in a process of transition from one system to another. How this happens is still fully obscure. Senft continues by discussing the inaccurate and partly misleading terminology used in analysing classifier languages. He points out that the term 'numeral classifier language' is partly inaccurate, because classifier morphemes in many languages are used also for other purposes, not only for counting animate and inanimate referents. He joins those researchers who have proposed 'numerative' as a generic term, which then subdivides into two distinct sub-classes, 'classifiers' and 'quantifiers'. There also occur such terms as 'sortal classifier' (individuates whatever it refers to in terms of the kind of entity that it is) and 'mensural classifier' (individuates in terms of quantity). In order to get more clarity to the use of categories, Senft proposes that semantically based groupings should be accepted only if they are grounded in the grammar and marked as being formally distinct in the respective language. Because classifications are not only formal, identifiable in linguistic forms, but basically also semantic, this has often resulted in grouping nouns of a language under different semantic labels, these groups constituting 'semantic systems' or 'semantic domains'. He proposes an analysis process, where the first and temporary semantic categorisation, made by the researcher, is reconsidered and amended later after having looked at their actual use in detail. Finally, the semantic categories should not be considered static but rather as dynamic domains which interact with the total system of classification. The brief description and analysis of the Kilivila classifier particles is given as a 'case study' for demonstrating in practice what was said before on a more general level. The argumentation is quite convincing as regards the Kilivila system and other similar systems. It is perhaps also an important contribution to the analysis of classifier languages, but less relevant in understanding other types of nominal classification systems. The second chapter also has a general scope. In it, Colette Grinevald gives a typology of classifiers on the basis of morphosyntactic properties. She places the typology of classifiers into the context of the more wide system of nominal classifications. Three major types of classifiers are distinguished, and the main purpose of the treatise is to 'produce a research tool that might foster the production of more thorough and more comparable descriptions of classifier systems'. The main motivation for working out a typology of classifiers emerges from confusion in work on classifiers. Because the use of terms by various researchers is not uniform, it has resulted in serious misunderstandings, especially when data are used by other researchers for comparative purposes. Grinevald places the classifiers in the middle of a continuum, where in its grammatical end are the gender and noun class systems, and in its lexical end are measure terms and class terms. Classifiers have a lexical origin and they are used in specific morphosyntactic constructions. They are different from lexical systems in that they mark categories of nouns by independent morphemes or by affixes that are attached to other words than the noun in question. They are also different from noun class systems and gender systems because of their incomplete grammaticalization. She identifies eight criteria through which noun class-gender systems differ from classifier systems. The main areal distribution of various systems is that gender systems are found in Indo-European languages, noun class systems in Niger- Congo languages (especially Bantu languages), and classifier systems in East and Southeast Asian languages. Within classifier languages she distinguishes such types as: (a) numeral classifiers (e.g. Thai and Burmese in Asia); (b) noun classifiers (e.g. Jakaltek and Akatek in Meso-America); (c) genitive classifier systems (Micronesian family in Oceania); (d) verbal classifiers (found in languages of North America). In addition to these categories that she calls 'prototypes' there is discussion on 'fuzzy edges', cases that do not fall to any of those categories. There is blending among the classifier systems themselves as well as blending with other nominal classification systems. Furthermore, there are systems, such as the Amazonian languages exemplify, that a language may have a gender system and another type of nominal classification system (resembling a noun class system) simultaneously. This would contradict the often-held view that the gender system and noun class system should be collapsed as one category. The typology proposed by Grinevald is based on morphosyntax. She realises the need of investigating the semantics and functions of the classifiers, 'before it will be possible to provide a functionally based comprehensive study of classifiers'. While acknowledging the main types of nominal classification, Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald gives a condensed account of such 'marginal' classifier types, which do not fall within those categories. Among such classifier types are classifier morphemes known by such names as 'demonstrative', 'article' and 'deictic' classifiers, which appear only with deictic elements. Tariana, a North Arawak language from Northern Amazonia, is an example of such a language. It uses almost the same classifier morphemes for such purposes as for verbal, possessive and noun classification, as well as numeral classifiers and noun class markers. The language also exhibits a distinction between classifiers with demonstratives and classifiers with articles. After a condensed (yet detailed) analysis of each of these classifier types a concluding summary of the properties and uses of classifiers in Tariana is given in a table. The chapter is clearly ordered and the argumentation is convincing, and its strength lies in that it concentrates on one language and tries to give a full account of its classification system. Although the book is on classification systems in general, this account on an individual language brings us down to earth and reminds us about the extreme complexity of systems which we have to deal with. Roberto Zavala describes multiple classifier systems in Akatek, which belongs to Mayan languages. Akatek has four different paradigms of classificatory devices, which in Zavala's terms are: classificatory suffixes, sortal numeral classifiers, plural for humans, and noun classifiers. What is interesting in this system is that more than one, even three, different classificatory morphemes can co-occur in the same nominal phrase. He claims that Akatek requires classifiers because the nouns of the language do not make distinction between singular and plural. Each of the four sets of noun classification has a double role. They can function as adnominal elements or as pro-forms that maintain the referent continuity in discourse. The use of classificatory suffixes depends on morphosyntactic factors, but the use of other three classificatory devices depends primarily on discourse-pragmatic factors. What is interesting is that, according to Zavala, none of the classificatory morphemes of those four sets of classifiers add new semantic information to the noun with which it combines. On the basis of phonological changes he proposes that the originally lexical items have become purely grammatical devices. Chapter 5 contains a reanalysis by David Wilkins' own earlier account of lexical noun classifiers of Mparntwe Arrernte, which belongs to the Pama-Nyungan languages in Central Australia. Noun classifiers are typically free morphemes standing next to the noun. Such morphemes are often considered polysemous, because in addition of being part of a nominal construction, they can also stand as nouns of their own. In this chapter Wilkins takes a different stand and, instead of treating noun classifiers as polysemous morphemes, he emphasizes that the classificatory morphemes should not be considered as nouns in classificatory contexts but rather as part of a larger construction, where the preceding morpheme expresses a semantically wider group, which the following more specific noun is a member of. Therefore, emphasis should not be in individual parts of the construction but rather in its semantic content. Therefore, although formally such a construction could be analysed as consisting of two adjacent nouns, where the former is generic in nature and the latter specifies it, semantically they constitute one single concept. The construction is comparable with multi-word terms, a phenomenon common in coining domain-specific terminology in most languages. So far twenty-six generic terms for Mparntwe Arrernte have been identified. And Wilkins suggests that any lexicalised superordinate term that has identifiable lexicalised hyponyms can occur as the generic in a generic-specific construction. Wilkins makes explicit what he calls his theoretical biases. He claims that generic nouns should not be treated as having one meaning as individual nouns and another meaning as part of generic-specific constructions - a situation common in many dictionaries. Rather, generic lexemes are monosemous, and the second 'meaning' is a function of the construction, not the property of the lexeme. He further claims that constructions themselves have a unique and non-compositional meaning. The third 'bias' concerns compositionality. It is claimed that while the meaning of a linguistic structure is a function of its parts and their manner of combination, this includes also that one of the 'parts' is the construction itself. The meaning of the construction is not a concatenative process, as if the meaning of one part would be added to the meaning of the other. Lexical, constructional and compositional sources of meaning should not be confused. This implies the fourth 'bias', namely the necessity of having a method of semantic description. What method should be employed is left open, lest one uses the method systematically. Finally Wilkins insists on the distinction between semantics and pragmatics. In building a model of the use of generic nouns in Arrernte Wilkins includes into it also a close discourse analysis and culturally grounded definitions. He also examines the use of such constructions in rhetorical speech as well as in creative and humorous contexts. When considering Arrernte data in comparison with other Australian languages, he concludes that most of them have merely generic-specific constructions without having developed true noun classifiers. Kyoko Inoue gives an account of the use of numeral classifiers in Japanese. Attention is paid especially to the extra-linguistic cultural factors in assigning classifiers to constructions. Results of research with children and people with various levels of skills in Japanese language show that classifier choice is not constant but it depends on several non-linguistic factors. One important factor in classifier choice seems to be visualizing ability, i.e. how speakers can mentally view and manipulate a noun. In fact constellations of cultural and social knowledge override formal and semantic factors. Jurgen Broschart argues in his contribution that the linguistic function of classifiers follows the principles of perception and cognition. Basing his argumentation of Gestalt Theory he tries to find common ground for discussion between different sciences of mind. He describes the function of classifiers and classifier-like elements in the context of the gestalt principles of the isolation of units and the unification of isolates. Numeral classifiers as well as mensural and collective classifiers define, and thus isolate, units of various kinds. Noun classifiers are concerned with 'naturally' independent, self-contained referents, while classifier-like constructions (possessive and locative) specify, and thus unify, 'unnatural' isolates. In trying to widen the perspective to other areas of the sciences of mind, Broschart contends that "there is no difference of principle between the function of a classifier in linguistics and e.g. the function of a concrete 'contour' or 'gestalt' in visual perception". The same claim applies to morphological noun class markers and gender markers, because they serve the purpose of identifying manipulable entities. The difference between noun classifiers and gender markers is the specification of domain. While noun classifiers operate on text level, specifying e.g. the topic of a text and are not compulsory, gender markers are obligatory for any form of a lexical noun. The gestalt operations (isolation of units and unification of isolates) are crucial for informing the hearer about the manipulability and identifiability of the parts of the domain in question. The noun class system, one of the major nominal classification systems, is discussed by Katherine Demuth in the chapter on Bantu noun class systems. This group of languages is perhaps the best-known representative of this classification system. Noun classes are not, however, treated here from a comparative viewpoint, for example by comparing them with other types of classification systems. The author has chosen a group- internal problem and she explores the nature of the semantic productivity and competing morphophonological processes as well as acquisition of the use of noun classes by children. The evidence is from such languages as Sesotho, Setswana, Zulu and Siswati. Demuth gives an outline of the Proto-Bantu noun class system (with 25 classes) and shows how some Bantu languages and languages distantly related to Bantu have retained noun classes in different degrees. She is interested in the question why some noun classes (especially 1/2 and 9/10) are more likely than others to persist in languages that lose classes. Various attempts to provide semantic basis for noun classes show that such a basis indeed exists, although the precise way of forming and labelling such groups is problematic. Some features, such as humanness and animacy, are clear criteria for noun class allocation, but in some other classes, such as 5/6 and 9/10, the semantic basis is much more complex. It is not clear either whether the noun classes should be seen to reflect semantic entities as distinct units or whether those units form a hierarchical structure in the way Denny and Creider (1986) have attempted. Demuth shows how Sesotho and Setswana are examples of different degrees of noun class productivity, especially in regard to locative classes. On the other hand, some other classes, e.g. the human class 1/2 seems to be productive in most Bantu languages, as are some other classes. Perhaps the most interesting part in this chapter is the loan word allocation, because this is done on the basis of competing criteria. The general rule in Sesotho seems to be that phonology and semantics are decisive, and when neither of these two applies, the noun is assigned to the default class, which in Sesotho is 9/10 and in Zulu 5/6. The study of the noun class acquisition by children shows that they may make grammatically motivated errors but not semantic overgeneralizations. The contribution of Demuth would be a fairly well documented piece of work on selected topics if she in the end had not ventured to contemplate the 'necessity' of noun classification. To compare noun classification with the classification of verbs with respect to tense/aspect is a far-fetched idea. That 'the classification of nouns is a semantic (and grammatical) necessity' is simply not true, because there are languages, for example in the Finno-Ugric family, which have no trace of nominal classification, if we exclude personal pronouns in 3rd person where a distinction is made between humans and non-humans. Greville Corbett and Norman Fraser give a logically concise account of gender assignment and demonstrate with the examples of Russian and Arapesh (Papua New Guinea) how a formal approach to gender typology suits for predicting gender allocation. They give a typology of gender systems on the basis of how nouns are assigned to genders, the basic types being semantic assignment, predominantly semantic assignment and formal systems (morphological assignment and phonological assignment). The approach of using formal properties for predicting gender assignment was tested by an inheritance formalism called DATR, developed for encoding Network Morphology. The argumentation is convincing. John Lucy has written a concluding chapter, where he pinpoints three conceptual and methodological confusions in the area and shows how they can be remedied. Especially he points out that we need to put emphasis on the analysis of local linguistic forms, especially on the patterns of formal meaning constructions chracteristic of the language in question.

    General comments

    The book claims to give an account of the state-of-the-art in the study of nominal classification in various languages. As far as no competitive account is available, as the case is, it has achieved this aim. It contains a wealth of data and scrupulous analysis from this very wide and difficult field. It aims at terminological clarity, and it has taken at least a step, if not a leap, into that direction. Although the level of contributions is somewhat uneven, I predict that it will become a standard reference work in this field for the years to come. My worry is that if distinction between such concepts as noun class system and gender system is blurred we lose the distinctiveness of those terms. Bantu languages are classical examples of a system where nouns are classified according to several semantic criteria, with no reference to sexual gender. Many other languages, including the Afro-Asiatic family, tend to classify nouns according to sexually defined gender. In some languages, as in the Nilo-Saharan Maasai, the gender division is very productive and nouns may switch between those two genders because of semantic purposes (male and big vs. female and small). It is likely that terminological distinctions will survive because of practical reasons.

    References

    Craig, Collette (ed.), 1986. Noun classes and categorization. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Denny, J.P. and C. Creider, 1986. The semantics of noun classes in Proto-Bantu. In C. Craig (ed.), Noun classes and categorization. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 217-39.

    Royen, Gerlach, 1929. Die nominalen Klassifikations-Systeme in den Sprachen der Erde. Historisch-kritische Studie, mit besonderer Beruecksichtigung des Indogermanischen. Anthropos Linguistische Bibliothek vol. 4. Vienna: Anthropos.

    Seiler, Hansjakob, 1986. Apprehension. Language, object and order, part 3: The Universal Dimension of Apprehension. Tuebingen: Narr.

    Seiler, Hansjakob, and Christian Lehman (eds.), 1982. Apprehension: das sprachliche Erfassen von Gegenstaenden, part I: Bereich und Ordnung der Phenomene. Tuebingen: Narr.

    Seiler, Hansjakob, and Franz-Joseph Stachowiak (eds.), 1982. Apprehension: das sprachliche Erfassen von Gegenstaenden, part 2: Die Techniken und ihr Zusammenhang in Einzelsprachen. Tuebingen: Narr.

    Unterbeck, Barbara and Rissanen, Matti (eds.), 1999. Gender in Grammar and Cognition. (Trends in Linguistics: Studies and Monographs 124). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.



    About the reviewer: Arvi Hurskainen, professor of African languages at the University of Helsinki, Finland. Main areas of interest: Bantu languages and linguistics, computational description (morphology, syntax and semantics) of Bantu languages (mostly Eastern and Southern), conceptual systems (esp. Maasai).