LINGUIST List 12.645

Thu Mar 8 2001

Review: Senft, Systems of Nominal Classification

Editor for this issue: Terence Langendoen <terrylinguistlist.org>


What follows is another discussion note contributed to our Book Discussion Forum. We expect these discussions to be informal and interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially invited to join in.

If you are interested in leading a book discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available for discussion." (This means that the publisher has sent us a review copy.) Then contact Simin Karimi at siminlinguistlist.org or Terry Langendoen at terrylinguistlist.org.


Directory

  • Wolfgang Schulze, Review of Senft (ed.) Nominal classification

    Message 1: Review of Senft (ed.) Nominal classification

    Date: Thu, 08 Mar 2001 17:18:58 +0100
    From: Wolfgang Schulze <W.Schulzelrz.uni-muenchen.de>
    Subject: Review of Senft (ed.) Nominal classification


    Senft, Gunter (ed.) (2000). Systems of Nominal Classification. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. vii, 350 pages.

    Claudia Gerstner-Link, University of Munich

    Research on Nominal Classification has a rather long standing tradition in Linguistics (starting with Gerlach Royen's well-known opus magnum 'Die nominalen Klassifikationssystem in den Sprachen der Welt' (1929)). Systems of nominal classification have attracted the interest of linguists not only because of their diverging morphosyntactic behavior and by universal features possibly underlying them, but also because they seem to offer a tool to relate linguistic categorization to principles and procedures of how human beings categorize (or learn to categorize) their worlds in a cognitive and/or social perspective. No wonder that research groups specialized in topics of cognitive linguistics such as the Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, now the Department of Language and Cognition at the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics (Nijmegen) have devoted much work on this promising subject. Gunter Senft, one of the members of this research group had organized a workshop 'Back to Basic Issues in Nominal Classification' in 1993 the output of which is - in parts - documented in the latest volume of Nominal Classification, namely in Senft (2000). This volume explicitly concentrates on the 'semantic' (or - in a rather broad sense - cognitive) foundations of nominal classification. It offers ten articles by renowned specialists in the topic to which is added an introduction by the editor (pp. 1-10) and an index (languages, subjects, names). Obviously, a single volume cannot comprehensively cover the empirics of the phenomenon. It has to waver between in-depth studies concentrating on single language data and a broader typological perspective. Fortunately enough, the authors concentrate on the single language (or contrastive) perspective and avoid a necessarily superficial cross-linguistic approach (mass comparison). Yet, the reader probably wants to know about the motives of the editor concerning the choice of languages that are presented in this volume. Is one of the motives the basic condition that the systems in question should have a 'conceptual-semantic basis rather than a formal basis' (p.1)? Or is the choice simply determined by the special research interests of the authors? The following languages are covered (more or less extensively) by the authors: Kilivala (Austronesian, Trobriand Islands) (Senft), Tariana (North-Arawak) (Aikhenbald), Akatek (Mayan) (Zavala), Arrernte (Pama-Nyungan) (Wilkins), Japanese (Inoue), Tongan (and other Oceanic languages) (Broschart), Bantu languages (Demuth), Russian, Quafar (East Cushitic), Ghodoberi (East Caucasian), Zande (Niger-Kordofanian) and Arapesh (Papuan: Torricelli) (Corbett & Fraser). Colette Grinevald's contribution offers data from a number of other languages (in a typological perspective). If we bear in mind that some languages such as Ghodoberi, Zande, or Quafar are treated in a rather superficial manner we arrive at the impression that the choice of languages is not very equilibrated and that it does not conform to the standards of typological sampling. I confess that editing a volume such as Senft's book generally necessitates some kind of compromising: Do I want to demonstrate the universe of nominal classification as it shows up in 'the languages of the world'? In this case, prominent language groups (or languages) should have been comprehensively monitored as for all relevant aspects of the phenomenon (including the diachronic perspective). Or: Are there arguments for a 'categorial' subdivision (and choice of data) which would show us the universe of nominal classification as such? I am left with the impression that the editor has taken the 'third' road, namely to leave the decision to the authors which systems (languages) are to be included in the volume. The scope of all ten papers is described in Senft's introduction. The first two papers (G. Senft: 'What do we really know about nominal classification systems', pp.11-49) and C. Grinevald: 'A morphosyntactic typology of classifiers', pp. 50-92) can be regarded as introductory sections on the semantics (Senft) and morphosyntax (Grinevald) of nominal classification systems. The next five papers are devoted to single language data from specific perspectives: A.Y. Aikhenvald: 'Unusual classifiers in Tariana', pp.93-113; R. Zavala: 'Multiple classifier systems in Akatek (Mayan), pp.114-146, D.F. Wilkins: 'Ants, ancestors and medicine: a semantic and pragmatic account of classifier constructions in Arrernte (Central Australian), pp.147-216; K. Inoue: 'Visualizing ability and nominal classification: evidence of cultural operations in the agreement rules of Japanese numeral classifiers', pp.217-238; J. Broschart: 'Isolation of units and unification of isolates: the gestalt-functions of classifiers', pp.239-269.The next two papers turn again to more general issues of noun classification: K. Demuth: 'Bantu noun class systems: loanword and acquisition evidence of semantic productivity', pp.270-292, G.G. Corbett & N.A. Fraser: 'Gender assignment: a typology and a model', pp.293-325. Finally, J. Lucy sums up the discussion ('Systems of nominal classification: a concluding discussion'). In this review, I cannot discuss all papers in detail. I will focus on the following ones: Senft, Grinevald, Aikhenvald, Zavala, Wilkins, and Lucy. The choice is determined by the assumption that these papers best represent the make-up and argumentative tendencies of the volume in question. In discussing the data from Kilivala (Austronesian), G. Senft tries to outline a psychologically and ethnolinguistically grounded approach to nominal classification. He interprets classification as a global means of human beings to interact with their environment into which linguistic classification is embedded. He enumerates (and exemplifies) the different techniques of linguistic classification and criticizes the fact that much too often such techniques are described as single events, but not in terms of a complex network structure which would allow to describe in more detail the interaction (or co-paradigmatization) of different classification techniques in one language. In order to pursue this question Senft elaborates the system of Kilivala, a so-called numeral classifying language. In criticizing the traditional division between qualifying (sortal) and quantifying (mensural) procedures, he discusses a feature based approach which focuses on the qualitative aspect (without telling us how qualitative features intervene with deictic and quantitative features). Senft describes classification systems as some kind of feature matrix the realization of which he characterizes as being culturally determined. Most importantly, the relation between nouns and classifiers is described as a dynamic (and variable) relation which is determined by discourse strategies. He arrives at a complex network that constitutes the semantics and functions of classifiers. Some classifiers seem to be strictly localized, other more variable which allows the speaker to pragmatically apply switching strategies. Senft concentrates on the description of communicative effects of dynamic classification but unfortunately refrains from looking at a cognitive motivation for this remarkable phenomenon. Grinevald's article presents a morphosyntactic typology of classifiers as a first step towards an overall grammatical typology of classifiers. There is no doubt that a classifier typology is needed as a heuristic tool to better understand the morphosyntactic and semantic-pragmatic intricacies of many complex classifier systems. Such a typology should be based on prototypes of classifier systems each for numeral classifiers, noun classifiers, genitive (possessive) classifiers, and verbal classifiers. These are the main classifier systems Grinevald is concerned with. In approaching the characteristics of classifiers she first locates classifiers in the middle of a continuum between lexical (measure and class terms) and grammatical (gender and noun classes) systems of nominal classification; then she outlines the major morphosyntactic properties of each classifier type. A decisive argument for the existence of different types of classifiers is the fact that languages may combine several classifier systems: Jakaltek is one of the best examples of a multiple classifier system. An important issue is the question whether a particular type of classifier correlates with special semantic features. As a working hypothesis Grinevald formulates the following correspondences: numeral classifiers are preferably associated with physical properties, noun classifiers with properties of material and essence, and genitive classifiers with functional properties. On the other hand, there is a functional similarity between all classifiers as they are a device of marking individuation of nouns. However, the nature of this process of individuation is far from being clear in its details and therefore urgently calls for further research. Particularly, it has to be thoroughly investigated how co-existing functional differences between the classifier types match their common property of individuation. Let us take the case of Jakaltek as an example: If noun classifiers are operators of quality, one faces the question how the assumption that Jakaltek noun classifiers function as determiners is in line with their semantics of qualification, since it can be argued that determination is at least equally related to quantification than to qualification. Thus, the future task is to develop in detail a functional typology of classifiers based on Grinevald's convincing insights and suggestions that certainly have a desirable and positive impact on any further studies of classifier systems. Aikhenvald's contribution deals with Tariana (North-Arawak). The author tries to explain the so-called 'unusual' system of nominal classification of Tariana in reference to the general classificatory procedures in this language. By 'unusual' is meant that 'class' is linked to deictic elements such as demonstrative pronouns and articles. This 'unusual' procedure is only one of the multiple (six fold) classificatory techniques of Tariana which are clearly elaborated in this paper (among them attribute noun concordance, numeral classifiers, classification of possessives, class markers on the verb with certain types of diathesis and subordination). The different strategies are finally summed up and evaluated in an impressing resume. The motivations of classifying strategies in Tariana are comprehensively monitored by the author; however, the argumentation lacks a more general answer to the question of what is the basic classificatory strategy (if there is one). Perhaps we could claim that the most prominent function of classification in Tariana is the grading of reference. Tariana allows a pragmatically motivated type of classification with deictic structures (optionally, context depended, fluid) which is opposed to a more semantic procedure of subcategorization (esp. noun classes and noun classifiers). The technique of article based classification seems to be operational between these two poles (deictic < article < noun class). The objectives of Wilkins's contribution are to revise the type of noun classifiers - itself a subtype of classifying constructions. He opposes the data from Arrernte to those of Yidiny and Jakaltek and convincingly elaborates the different functional properties in the three languages. Arrernte has generic nouns which classify nouns according to their inherent semantics, functions, and usage. The corresponding constructions are called generic-specific constructions. The choice of the appropriate classifier depends on the linguistic and communicative (situative) context, in other words, it is fluid. In Jakaltek, on the contrary, such a classification is strongly linked to determination and individuation. Yidiny, too, lacks this aspect of determination; however, in this language there seems to be no difference between generic-specific constructions and noun phrases consisting only of specific nouns. In Arrernte, isolated specific nouns are embedded into a discourse pattern different from that of their use within generic-specific constructions. They also call for syntactic and semantic-pragmatic features that stand in complementary distribution to those of generic-specific constructions. With respect to the Arrernte data it seems crucial to describe the semantics of generic nouns. In order to tackle this task, Wilkins makes use of Wierzbicka's conceptual analysis and Fillmore's Frame Semantics. He shows that a specific noun receives its interpretational frame by means of a generic noun, or: the semantics of a generic-specific construction results from the semantics of the two components and their configurational structure. The technique of classification shows up as a configuration without having true classifiers. In other words: it is an emergent phenomenon. Zavala's article deals with Akatek (Mayan) as an example of a multiple classifier system which contains four different classifier paradigms. A noun phrase may exhibit up to three different classifiers, but is only the numeral classifiers that are obligatory. If we have a structure consisting of a numeral plus numeral classifier plus plural classifier plus noun classifier plus noun each of the three classifiers expresses a particular aspect concerning the individuation of the noun: the exact quantity, plurality, and referentiality. Noun classifiers may occur as the only element that combines with a noun or together with other modifiers or classificatory devices. However, it remains unclear how word order is determined in complex noun phrases: does the numeral suffixed by a numeral classifier precede or follow the noun classifier? Apparently both ordering principles are possible. But note that a middle position of numerals in the NP generally can only be found, if the preceding elements are articles or demonstratives, that is, if the whole phrase is definite. Thus, example (39)c. would especially support a determiner like reading of noun classifiers as it happens to be the case in Jakaltek. By contrast, the question remains what exactly is the function of the noun classifier in a structure where it follows the numeral expression (cf. examples (1), (15), (21), (22)). Unfortunately, Zavala does not discuss this interesting puzzle. The main impression one gets from the Akatek classifier system is that it is a system in transition that should be further studied in order to reveal the stages of development of classifiers systems into determiner systems. Lucy's discussion of the state of the art of analyzing classifier systems is very elucidatory in that he clearly indicates the still weak points in the overall conception of this topic. Is it experience or linguistic form that is classified? Another essential issue is the "actual logic uniting all forms of nominal classification, namely their contribution to adequate noun phrase reference." (p.329) This referential function of any kind of nominal classification should be much more focused on in future research. In determining what the classifiers contribute to reference in a particular language and construction one must not become biased by translation projections. This can be prevented by reflecting the three different functional components of a noun phrase given by Silverstein: the intrinsic type of the referent, its extrinsic individuation status, and its discourse presupposability. From this vantage Lucy concludes "that classifiers of different types form a gradient from those dealing with relatively intrinsic qualities of a referent, to those dealing with relatively extrinsic qualities of a referent, to those dealing with relatively transient pragmatic contingencies of the speech event." (p.339) Thus, the (morpho)syntactic position of a classifier in a noun phrase is crucial to understand its exact function. Although all articles of the book present interesting results or give rise to substantial questions within the field it is Grinevald's view that comes closest to Lucy's in arguing for a more elaborated functional, i.e. semantic and cognitive, perspective in debating the topic of nominal classification.

    To conclude: The present volume is a welcome contribution to the study of nominal classification both in a typological and a semantic perspective. It owes much to the highly informed authors and the specific phenomena of the languages presented in the ten papers. Once the reader has worked through the huge amount of material and analyses (s)he will gain a deeper insight into the morphosyntactic and semantic strategies of establishing reference. It will be upon future researchers to draw their conclusions from the analyses and interpretations proposed in the different paper's and to test them against their own data. In this sense, Senft's volume figures as an important step towards what is urgently in want but not yet in sight: a universal and typologically validated theory of the cognitive motivation and pragmatic (communicative) power of linguistic classification.

    Dr. Claudia Gerstner-Link is a Research Fellow at the Institute for General and Typological Linguistics of the University of Munich. After extensive fieldwork in Papua New Guinea, she is currently preparing a major book on Kilmeri, a hitherto undescribed Papuan language. Her research interests are Papuan linguistics, Formal and Functional Typology, and Cognitive Linguistics.

    Editors' note: This review was originally requested of Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Schulze, Institut f�r Allgemeine und Indogermanische Sprachwissenschaft Universit�t M�nchen. With our permission, he asked Dr. Gerstner-Link to prepare the review.