LINGUIST List 12.930

Tue Apr 3 2001

Review: McNeill, Language and Gesture

Editor for this issue: Terence Langendoen <terrylinguistlist.org>


What follows is another discussion note contributed to our Book Discussion Forum. We expect these discussions to be informal and interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially invited to join in.

If you are interested in leading a book discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available for discussion." (This means that the publisher has sent us a review copy.) Then contact Simin Karimi at siminlinguistlist.org or Terry Langendoen at terrylinguistlist.org.


Directory

  • Nicla Rossini, Review of McNeill, Language and Gesture

    Message 1: Review of McNeill, Language and Gesture

    Date: Tue, 3 Apr 2001 22:22:15 +0200
    From: Nicla Rossini <tattvamasilibero.it>
    Subject: Review of McNeill, Language and Gesture


    McNeill, David, ed. (2000) Language and Gesture, Cambridge University Press, 409 pp. ISBN 0 521 77166 8 (hardback) 0 521 77761 5 (paperback)

    Reviewed by Nicla Rossini, Department of Linguistics, University of Pavia, Italy.

    The book is a collection of papers written after a conference entitled "Gestures Compared Cross-Linguistically" held in 1995 in Albuquerque. Anyway, it is not a proceedings and all the authors wrote their contributes between 1996 and 1997. It covers a wide range of topics offering a strong cross-linguistic and cross-cultural view of non verbal communication.

    The introduction (by David McNeill) zeroes in on the nature of gesture and the necessity of distinguishing between different kinds of movements we usually call with the same name: on this purpose, he recalls Kendon (1982), who singles out four relevant points - "gesticulation", "pantomime", "emblem" and "sign language" - within the non verbal phenomenon and continues on giving his own interpretation of gesture focusing on its relationship to speech, linguistic properties, convention and semiosis. Each kind of relationship is a way to single out a continuum in which gestures take place - in other words, a classification model.

    Lastly, the editor gives an account of antecedents in the study of gesture and points to the different approaches (such as social interaction, cognitive psychology, modeling and so on) that the book's structure is going to highlight.

    Part One - Gesture in action

    1. Pointing, gesture spaces, and mental maps (John Haviland) We usually consider pointing gestures as an unproblematic matter because of the objectivity of space and environment: this chapter, on the contrary, claims that space is itself a construction and, subsequently, different cultures may have different space models. The author describes in a close way the occurrences of pointing gestures in the Mayan culture of Tzotzil and the Australian Aborigine culture of Guugu Yimithirr to conclude that gesture spaces are "complex constructions" implying both geographic and social features.

    2. Language and gesture: unity or duality? (Adam Kendon) In this chapter, Kendon broaches a classical question in gesture studies: should we consider gesture and language as different and independent phenomena? Basing his own answer on a good number of examples that show different roles (such as pragmatic, contextualizing, propositional, and so on) that co-verbal gesture may play in communication, the author claims a "unity" for language and gesture, since "gesture and speech, as used in conversation, serve different but complementary roles".

    3. The influence of addressee location on spatial language and representational gestures of direction (Asli Oezyuerek) A really interesting study on the ways in which language and gesture achieve spatial reference: according to this author, spatial language and gesture are motivated not only by spatial reference, but also by social context. The results of two studies on influence of addressee location on spatial language and gesture have shown that it changed gesture orientation but didn't change speech expression. Gesture changing, however, is not simply related to addressee location, but also, and more strongly, to the meanings expressed in speech: the gesturer used the "shared space" in a different way when he was expressing, for example, "in", "out" or "across" motions. These results imply that neither speech nor gesture alone is sensitive to addressee location, but the whole process of communication is made up of speech and gesture.

    4. Gesture, aphasia, and interaction (Charles Goodwin) It is a study on the way gestures vehicle their message: should we argue that the only one hand movement is a meaningful action? In order to give an answer to this question, Goodwin considers the case of Chil, a seriously aphasic man who is able to speak only three words (Yes, No, and And) because of a massive stroke in the left hemisphere of his brain. Chil, however, is able to communicate with his relatives using limited gestures. Analyzing his strategies for communication, the author focuses on the role of gestures tying (in parallel with gesture-speech tying) in social interaction.

    5. Gestural interaction between the instructor and the learner in origami instruction (Nobuhiro Furuyama) "Do people gesturally interact with each other?" This is the question that the author tries to answer in this chapter, in which an experiment on origami (the Japanese paper-folding art) experience is reported: eighteen students were divided in pairs where the more experienced one had to instruct his colleague in the construction of an origami balloon with no use of paper or any kind of utensils. Of course, a great amount of gestures were performed, both by the instructor and by the learner: of great interest, learners' collaborative gestures (those which interacted with the gesture of the partner, even manipulating parts of it).

    6. Gestures, knowledge, and the world (Curtis Le Baron and Jurgen Streeck) This chapter is intended to investigate the function of social shared knowledge in gesture production by the analysis of communication in "material-rich learning settings" such as do-it-yourself of architecture classrooms. The study shows how gestures acquire gradually independent meanings to a specific community.

    Part Two - Gesture in thought

    7. Growth points in thinking-for-speaking (David McNeill and Susan D. Duncan) In this chapter, McNeill's Growth points theory (see also McNeill, 1992) is explained. The authors begin their work with a short presentation of gesture's own semiotic properties, and, afterwards, with a careful outline of growth points that they consider, following Vygotsky (1987), as the minimal psychological units. The final aim is to consider the function of co-verbal gestures in three languages (namely, English, Spanish, and Chinese) focusing on the semantic domain of motion. The moral is that different languages create different modes of thinking-for-speaking and, subsequently, different strategies for meaning representations in speech and gesture.

    8. How representational gestures help speaking (Sotaro Kita) The aim of this chapter is to discuss the cognitive function of representational gestures (that McNeill, 1992, divides into "iconic" and "abstract deictic" gestures): do they help lexical retrieval (see the Lexical Retrieval Hypothesis - see Rauscher et al. 1996), or maintaining an image (see Freedman 1977)? All of these hypotheses is based on a reasonable number of examples from human speaking behavior, so, the author suggests an alternative view of the phenomenon, namely, the Information Packaging Hypothesis, based on the interaction of spatio-motoric thinking and analytic thinking. The production of representational gestures would help speakers in organizing spatio-motoric information into suitable packages for linguistic expression.

    9.Where do most spontaneous representational gestures actually occur with respect to speech? (Shuici Nobe) An interesting research on a classic of gesture studies: are we able to determine a kind of regularity in speech-gesture timing? And, if yes, what kind of regularity? Nobe's study focuses on representational gestures: according to Nobe's experiment results, most of that kind of gestures would initiate during speech articulation and not during silent pauses (as in Beattie and Aboudan, 1994).

    10. Gesture production during stuttered speech: insights into the nature of gesture-speech integration (Rachel I. Mayberry and Joselynne Jaques) One of the mostly widespread assumptions about non verbal communication is that of a larger gestural production in case of speech lexical retrieval difficulty. This chapter is going to prove this is not true in case of stuttering: gesture, on the contrary, is co-produced with fluent speech but not with disfluent one. What's more, only speech related gestures are disrupted by stuttering: in fact, stuttering affected subjects were asked to perform some manual tasks while speaking, and all of them were able to perform his task even during stuttering attack. These data suggest a common deep principle of co-expression for speech and co-verbal gestures.

    11. The role of gestures and other graded language forms in the grounding of reference in perception (Elena T. Levy and Carol A. Fowler) This chapter points out to the pragmatic perspective of nonverbal communication: its aim is to demonstrate that there is no arbitrary relation between meanings and communicative activities' properties. Readers' attention is driven towards some basic concepts, such as reference, origo, topic structure (see Buehler 1990), and metanarrative statements (McNeill, 1992) in which an origo shifting usually occurs. During metanarrative statements (and subsequent topic shift) "energy peaks" (in the speech flow as well as in gesturing) would occur, in order to help addressee's comprehension task with respect to new referents to be introduced in narration. The question is whether listeners make use of this kind of redundant information to reduce their dependence on lexical and grammatical forms conveying the same pieces of information.

    12. Gesture and the transition from one-to two-word speech: when hand and mouth come together (Cynthia Butcher and Susan Goldin-Meadow) A study on children gesture-speech combination to determine whether gesture and speech form an integrated system: in this perspective, an experiment on six children is made. The children are video-taped in their homes for months: the videotaping begins when they are in the one-word period of language development and continues until they produce two words combinations. In one-word period gestures appear not to be perfectly integrated with speech and gestures are sometimes produced without speech: even in gesture-speech combinations, gesture was initially not synchronous with speech. Next, gesture and speech become more fully integrated. This would suggest that gestural communication evolves together with speech: another likely proof of a gesture-speech common psycho-motoric origin.

    Part Three - Modeling gesture performance

    13. Lexical gestures and lexical access: a process model (Robert M. Krauss, Yihsiu Chen, and Rebecca F. Gottesman) This chapter offers a new model for gesture interpretation: the first claim is that, instead of considering the communicative value of gesture (this kind of view have provided only partial answers to our questions), we should assume that different kinds of gestures have different origins and play different roles. The authors' attention is focused on a specific kind of gestures, namely lexical gestures, also called "representational gestures" (see McNeill, Cassell and McCullough 1994). In outlining their model, they follow Levelt's one (1989) for speech production: Levelt singles out three stages for the speaking process, namely, "conceptualizing", "formulating", and "articulating". According to the authors of this chapter, gesture origin would be at the conceptualizing stage, namely, the speaker's working memory would originate the representations to be reflected in lexical gestures. Thus, lexical gestures would reflect representations in memory.

    14. The production of gesture and speech (Jan Peter de Ruiter) Another model for gestures' origin and functions understanding, in this case, using the Information Processing Approach. Even in this case, the author bases his model on Levelt's (1989) one. The author assumes that gesture is a communicative device from the speaker's point of view (in other words, gesture is seen as an extremely communicative act, no matter of the real effectiveness of gestural communication). Afterwards, the Sketch Model Approach is outlined: once again, the conceptualizer is seen as responsible for gesturing process initiation. Sketch is the selection of pieces of information to be expressed in gesture; then a motor program is generated and is, lastly, executed. Obviously, the generation of gestures is closely related (say "coupled") with speech generation: the generation of gestures is implied by the creation of a preverbal sketch model. The Sketch Model, lastly, is an attempt to consider and explain many findings in gesture-speech area with a modular Information Processing Model.

    15. Catchments and contexts: non-modular factors in speech and gestures production (David McNeill) This chapter joins chapter number seven to present the concept of growing points. In this case a comparison with Information Processing models is made. Growth points theory is claimed to be more helpful in gestures interpretation due to its capability of including context as a component of speaking. It also predicts speech-gesture timing based on the content of utterances. Modular models, on the contrary don't seem to be sufficiently flexible for gesture communication analysis.

    Part Four - From gesture to sign

    16. Blended spaces and deixis in sign language discourse (Scott K. Liddell) It's the analysis of space use in American Sign Language. As we know, signs are produced in a certain location in space: a few classes of signs can be performed in an unlimited number of locations (the meaning remains the same notwithstanding the sign's direction in space), but, for a great number of signs, spatially direction is meaningful. Starting from Klima and Bellugi's theory, which he calls "Horizontal Plane Theory" and considers not so precise in signs direction's analysis, the author continues on introducing new concepts, such as "surrogates" and "tokens": a surrogate is meant to be an entity imagined to be present; whereas a token (see Liddell 1994) - thus representing an entity too - is more likely a shapeless area ahead of the signer. While tokens' size is usually small, a surrogate can be "full-sized", and, what's more, while one could talk about tokens, but not to them, surrogates may play the interlocutor's role. During speech-gesture performance, tokens and surrogates - that are part of the signer's mental space - can be mapped onto Real Space to create a blended Real Space.

    17. Gestural precursors to linguistic constructs: how input shapes the form of language (Jill P. Morford and Judy A. Kegl) It's a study on deaf children first language acquisition, with special regard for the role of input: deaf children, in fact, can be exposed to different kind of input in visual modality, namely, to gesture, homesign, non native signing, and native signing, different kinds of input being crucial - together with children's age - to determine a good (or bad) language competence. An interesting study on Idioma de Senas de Nicaragua is also proposed, in order to determine how much idiosyncratic gestures (homesigns) shaped the new-born signed language.

    18. Gesture to sign (language) (William C. Stokoe) This chapter proposes a new kind of interpretation of language origin: language may have begun with gestural expression. Instrumental manual actions may have been transformed in symbolic gestures, and Vision would have been the key of language evolution: humans would have begun to represent the world they would see (namely, things and actions) by their own means. Vision would have been the key for syntax to slowly come up because of its great capability of parallel processing.

    The book offers a wide range of arguments for discussion and future studies: due to the structure itself, it may result somewhat unsystematic, although contents turn out to be a precious contribution to our field. In fact, it provides a good global view of the main questions that scholars have been asking themselves in these years, namely: - what's the psychological origin of gesture? - which are its functions within speech interaction? - is gesture performed in order to help listener's comprehension, or may it also help speaker's task? The answers to these questions are still controversial: even the nature of gesture itself is argument of discussion. This book chooses a view of gesture as fully joined with verbal communication, even bearing the same psychological origin and complementary functions with respect to speech flow, but not all of scholars are convinced of this thesis: I could cite, for example, Butterworth & Hadar (1989), or Feyereisen and Seron (1982), who consider gesture production as accessory with respect to speech flow. Anyway, I think the greater part of researchers agree with the kind of model here proposed. As regards the problems related to the function of gesture in communication, I agree with de Ruiter's assumption that gestures, though not maintaining the same communicative efficacy under all circumstances, must be held as a strictly communicative device from the speaker's point of view: I would even add that the real nature of gesture becomes fully comprehensible only if we consider it as an inseparable part of a process - that I call "oral communication" - which is solely intentional, and consequently, strictly communicative. As for the rest, I particularly appreciated the constant attempt to focus on the social aspect of non verbal communication which is not - as it may seem - a homogeneous and objective phenomenon. I also found some chapters really original and interesting, like Oezyuerek's or Furuyama's ones, for example, and wish I could be able to read something more about their current studies: the greatest limit of this book is, in fact, its fragmentation into several topics (each one nearly covering a one chapter space), which may also be considered its best value.

    References

    Beattie, G. and Aboudan, R. 1994. Gestures, pauses and speech: an experimental investigation of the effects of changing social context on their precise temporal relationships. Semiotica 99:239-272. Butterworth, B. and Hadar, U. 1989. Gesture, speech, and computational stages: a reply to McNeill. Psychological Review, 96 I:168-174. Buehler, K. 1990. Theory of Language: The Representational Function of Language. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Feyereisen, P. e Seron, X. 'Nonverbal Communication and Aphasia: a Review. II. Expression'. Brain and Language,16:213-236. Freedman, N. 1977. Hands, words, and mind: on the structuralization of body movements during discourse and the capacity for verbal representation. In Freedman, N. and Grand, S. (eds.) 1977. Communicative Structures and Psychic Structures: A Psychoanalytic Interpretation of Communication. New York: Plenum. Kendon, A. 1982. The study of gesture: some remarks on its history. Semiotic Inquiry 2:45-62. Levelt, W. J. M. 1989. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT press. Liddell, S. K. 1994. Tokens and surrogates. In Ahlgren, I., Bergman, B. and Brennan, M. (eds.) 1994. Perspectives on Sign Language Research. Vol. I. University of Durham: International Sign Linguistics Association. McNeill, D. 1992. Hand and Mind: what gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. McNeill, D., Cassell, J. and McCullough, K.-E. 1994. Communicative effects of speech-mismatched gestures. Language and Social Interaction 27:223-237. Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M. and Chen, Y. 1996. Gesture, speech, and lexical access: the role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7:226-230. Vygotsky, L. S. 1987. Thinking and speech. Ed. R. W. Rieber and A. S. Carton. In the collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, vol. I: Problems of General Psychology, pp. 39-285.

    The reviewer: Nicla Rossini, a Ph.D. student in Linguistics at the Department of Linguistics, University of Pavia, Italy. I'm working on gesture's cognitive origin by the analysis of deaf and hearing people's non verbal communication.