LINGUIST List 13.2166

Sat Aug 24 2002

Review: General Ling: Heine & Kuteva (2002)

Editor for this issue: Naomi Ogasawara <naomilinguistlist.org>


What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially invited to join in.

If you are interested in leading a book discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available for review." Then contact Simin Karimi at siminlinguistlist.org.


Directory

  • Timur Maisak, Heine & Kuteva (2002) World Lexicon of Grammaticalization

    Message 1: Heine & Kuteva (2002) World Lexicon of Grammaticalization

    Date: Fri, 23 Aug 2002 18:55:38 +0400 (MSD)
    From: Timur Maisak <timikisabase.philol.msu.ru>
    Subject: Heine & Kuteva (2002) World Lexicon of Grammaticalization


    Heine, Bernd, and Tania Kuteva (2002) World Lexicon of Grammaticalization. Cambridge University Press, 387pp, Hardback ISBN 052180339-X, $70.00

    Announced in http://linguistlist.org/issues/12/12-1983.html

    Timur A. Maisak, Institute of Linguistics, Russian Academy of Sciences

    OVERVIEW & SYNOPSIS

    "World Lexicon of Grammaticalization" (henceforth simply the Lexicon, or WLG) summarizes the most salient generalizations that have been made on the unidirectional change of grammatical forms and constructions. It is a product of ten years of research carried out at the University of Cologne and the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences (Stanford, California), and its main purpose is to make accessible to a wider readership a wealth of data on the origin and development of grammatical forms that has been published over the course of the last three decades. This is a reference work in A-Z format; in total the Lexicon covers about 400 processes relating to the evolution of grammatical categories, using data from roughly 500 different languages.

    The findings delineated in the book are relevant to students of language across theoretical boundaries. They may be of help for diachronic reconstruction, "especially in areas where other tools available to the historical linguist, such as the comparative method and internal reconstruction, do not yield appropriate results" (p.1). Descriptive linguists will find in this book information "on how and why different grammatical meanings can be related to one another in a principled way, (...) on why there are some regular correspondences between grammatical forms and the meanings expressed by them, or on why certain linguistic forms have simultaneously lexical and grammatical functions" (p.1). Although potential readership of the book includes first of all linguists, the authors note that the Lexicon may be of some interest also to anthropologists, sociologists, and psychologists, who "may discover that the kind of human behavior held responsible for the evolution of grammatical forms is not all that different from the kind of behavior they observe in their own fields of study" (p.1).

    In the INTRODUCTION (pp. 1-14) the authors provide a brief characteristics of their work, its theoretical background (which is modern grammaticalization theory) and problems they encountered while compiling the Lexicon. Grammaticalization is conceived in WLG as "above all a semantic process" (p.3), so in this book only meaning of linguistic forms is concerned, and their morphosyntactic properties are normally not discussed. It is also acknowledged that grammaticalization is a unidirectional process (with rare exceptions), so all the paths are listed in a strict "X > Y" format with definite directionality. At the same time, some possible cases exhibiting alternative directionality are also mentioned (for example from Chinese and Russian), although not all of the alleged counterexamples seem convincing to me (see point 5 of the Discussion below).

    There was obviously a wide range of problems the authors had to take care of, and they discuss the most serious difficulties in the second part of the Introduction: for example, the findings are based on data only from "hardly more than one-tenth of the world's languages" (p.5); the continuous (chainlike) nature of grammaticalization processes has to be presented in a discrete "source > target" format, which is certainly a simplification; the quality of the data wholly depends on the information contained in available published sources, and this information may often be unsatisfactory; the very question of when one can definitely say that grammaticalization has been concluded is far from being an easy one, and so on. Of course, the presence of all these problems does not make the whole endeavour impossible, and I can say that the decisions the authors of WLG make in difficult cases seem to be in general deliberate and convincing. All the controversial points are commented upon in the Lexicon entries; some interpretations which I do not find very felicitous will be treated in more detail below.

    The second introductory chapter (pp. 15-26) presents a classification of GRAMMATICAL CONCEPTS (functions) used in the book. The terms used are mainly the same that are found in modern literature on grammatical typology; some terminological variants are also provided. Additional labels in parentheses refer to superordinate categories: cf. ABLATIVE (spatial, case), BENEFACTIVE (case), HABITUAL (aspect), EPISTEMIC (modality), etc. The presence of such concepts as ALREADY, ALSO, EVEN, EXCLAMATION, NEXT, ONLY, ONE, TWO, OTHER, SAME, SOME, SUCCEED, TOGETHER may seem surprising at a first glance, but the authors state explicitly that they made no attempt to trace a strict bound between 'grammatical' and 'non-grammatical or 'lexical' concepts and the reason for including such concepts as ONE or TOGETHER in the Lexicon is simply because these items often "exhibit more grammatical properties, or fewer lexical properties, than the concepts from which they are historically derived" (p.15). The list of concept labels, with approximate glosses and some descriptive notes, includes in total about 170 items.

    The main part of the book is the SOURCE-TARGET LEXICON (pp. 27-316), where about 400 processes of grammatical change are discussed (below I will also use the term "grammaticalization paths", although the authors seem to avoid it for some reason). The Lexicon includes numerous cases of changes from lexical source to grammatical concept (e.g. BELLY > IN, CHILD > DIMINUTIVE, COME TO > FUTURE, EAT > PASSIVE, GIVE > BENEFACTIVE, SAY > EVIDENTIAL, SIT > CONTINUOUS, THING > COMPLEMENTIZER, YESTERDAY > PAST, and so on), as well as from one grammatical concept to another (like ABLATIVE > AGENT, ALLATIVE > DATIVE, COMITATIVE > INSTRUMENT, CONDITIONAL > CONCESSIVE, FUTURE > EPISTEMIC MODALITY, LOCATIVE > A-POSSESSIVE ('of', attributive possession), PERFECT > PAST, H-POSSESSIVE ('have', predicative possession) > OBLIGATION, REFLEXIVE > PASSIVE, etc.). In each case, suitable examples from a variety of languages are given, and references to the relevant research literature are provided. Many entries also contain a brief discussion of cognitive motivation underlying the path of development, with reference to similar types of paths.

    The book is supplemented by three APPENDIXES: * the "Source-Target List" summarizes the Lexicon, listing alphabetically all paths in a "Source > Target" format; * the "Target-Source List" is organized in a similar manner, and is especially useful, as it allows to find all known historical sources for a given grammatical marker; for example, one can find out that grammatical marker for AGENT may arise from ABLATIVE, COMITATIVE, HAND (lexical source) or LOCATIVE, and the marker of CAUSATIVE may have one of lexical verbs DO, GIVE or TAKE as its source; * the alphabetical "List of Languages" enumerates more than 500 language names with information about their genetic affiliation.

    An extensive list of References (on 36 pages) concludes the book.

    DISCUSSION

    In my opinion, WLG is certainly one of the most important books concerning grammaticalization, although it mainly does not contain new data about the world's languages but rather systematizes and summarizes what is already known from other sources. The necessity of such kind of work has always been felt, because such a great number of facts about grammatical changes in individual languages is dispersed and scattered in grammatical descriptions or is discussed on pages of numerous articles and theoretical monographs.

    Although there are quite a few monographs on grammaticalization theory published in last 20 years (beginning with Lehmann (1982)), WLG seems to be the first comprehensive reference work listing hundreds of grammaticalization paths with examples from half a thousand languages. In fact, it was Bernd Heine who promoted the compilation of a lexicon of grammaticalization already at the beginning of the "grammaticalization renaissance" in early 1980s. The first attempt to enumerate grammaticalization paths (found in African languages) from source to target and from target to source is present in the Appendix to Heine & Reh (1984). Nine years later a "Lexicon of grammaticalization processes in African languages" (Heine et al. (1993)) appeared, which included also data from languages outside Africa and which is a direct predecessor of WLG. However, WLG which is published nine more years later includes data on a still wider range of languages and provides useful comments about individual paths of change, which were absent in the earlier version. (At the same time, it seems that not all the data from Heine et al. (1993) was carried to WLG, so these two books are to a much extent independent one from another.)

    As WLG was conceived just as a reference work, with the main purpose to collect the information available from other sources, it does not contain a general discussion of results -- like relative frequency of different paths (or groups of paths), or preferences of these or those types of changes in particular languages, etc. (although such information can normally be found in the authors' notes to individual entries). However, on the basis of data available from WLG, the reader can make his or her own conclusions about such matters.

    To give an idea about relative frequency of different sources and targets I would like to present here some results of my counts based on the summarizing lists of processes in the Appendixes. As it was mentioned before (and as it is stated by the authors themselves on p.1), total number of "unidirectional grammatical processes" described in the book approximates 400. According to the Appendixes, these processes involve 182 possible source items and 144 targets -- this correlation alone seems to well confirm the observation that "usually a language has several options to choose from in order to introduce a new category, or to replace an existing one" (Heine & Reh 1984: 113).

    The "Top 12" that follows shows which source items are the most frequent, according to WLG. Below I list those items that can give rise to more than 5 target concepts (the number of possible paths is given in brackets): * LOCATIVE (11), * COMITATIVE (10), * GET (9), ONE (9), * DEMONSTRATIVE (8), SAY (8), * ABLATIVE (7), ALLATIVE (7), GO (7, with also 3 paths for GO TO), TAKE (7), * COPULA (6, with also 5 paths for COPULA, LOCATIVE), LEAVE (6).

    The verb COME is also worth mentioning, as it may be a source for 4 concepts, and there are also 5 paths for COME TO and 2 paths for COME FROM listed separately. So, if we speak about most widely used primary, i.e. lexical, sources, verbs -- or, more correctly, verbal meanings -- COPULA (and LOCATIVE COPULA), GO (and GO TO), COME (also COME TO, COME FROM), GET, SAY, TAKE and LEAVE have the highest ranks.

    Another list which I made up comprises top 15 targets, i.e. grammatical concepts which can arise from several -- at least more than 5 (this is a number I chose arbitrarily) -- different sources: * CONTINUOUS (13), * FUTURE (12), * CAUSE (9), PASSIVE (9), * COMPARATIVE (8), COMPLEMENTIZER (8), HABITUAL (8), PURPOSE (8), * CLASSIFIER (7), OBLIGATION (7), A-POSSESSIVE (7), H-POSSESSIVE (7), TEMPORAL (7), * AFTER (6), DOWN (6), FRONT (6).

    The total number of 400 grammaticalization paths looks considerable enough, the impression being strengthened by the fact that among the paths listed in WLG very rare or peculiar ones can be found (like ALONE > ONLY, BEAT > PRO-VERB, BOWELS > IN (spatial), BRANCH > CLASSIFIER, EAR > LOCATIVE, FIELD > OUT, FOOTPRINT > BEHIND, MAN > EXCLAMATION, SEE > PASSIVE, etc.). However, that does not mean that the list presented in WLG is by any means exhaustive. It is very hard, of course, to conjecture what part of all the possible -- or at least the most frequent -- paths is covered in the book (90 percent, or 50, or 30?). But it is obvious that this number can, and should be, increased. Paths of change which have occurred to me while reading WLG and which, to my opinion, are worthy of being added to the Lexicon (although they are not mentioned there now), include, for example:

    * LOOK AT [+ Verb] > CONATIVE ('try to'), at least in Turkic and Dravidian; * SAY [+ Num] > ORDINAL (ordinal number marker), at least in Northeast Caucasian; * RETURN [+ Verb] > REFLEXIVE (see Lichtenberk (1991) for discussion of this and other developments from RETURN, GO and COME in Oceanic languages); * GO [+ Verb, past participle] > PASSIVE, at least in Indo-Iranian and Italian; * COME [+ Verb, past participle] > PASSIVE, at least in Indo-Iranian, Italian and Maltese; * COME [+ Verb] > DESIDERATIVE ('want to'), at least in Turkic; * COME [+ Verb] > CAUSATIVE, at least in Icelandic and Swedish; * ENTER [+ Verb] > INGRESSIVE; * GO UP [+ Verb] > INGRESSIVE.

    Much more grammaticalization paths for motion verbs can be listed; many of them are described in Maisak (2002), a study based on data from more than 200 languages. A brief discussion of possible changes of EAT to grammatical marker -- on p.122 of WLG only "EAT > PASSIVE" is mentioned -- can be found in Naess (2001).

    Absence of mention of the development of negative particles from nouns (like in case of French "pas" or "point" in "ne ... pas" construction) is also somewhat unexpected.

    The authors of the Lexicon are certainly aware of the fact that "what is covered in the book might represent merely the tip of the iceberg of what future generations of researches might discover" (p.13). Already in the Introduction, they admit that the data presented in WLG "constitute but a fraction of all instances of presumed or actual grammaticalization that we were confronted with" (p.12), and that they normally did not include paths of development where only examples from one language family were available, or those where the reconstruction of the processes was not reliable enough.

    The present work of Bernd Heine and Tania Kuteva cannot be overestimated, because (as it was already stated above) it is a unique and comprehensive lexicon of grammaticalization phenomena (with numerous examples and comments) compiled so far. My most strong wish is that this Lexicon should be developed further, as a great number of grammatical changes is still to be revealed and described. I think the best way to continue this project would be to make it open to additions and corrections from specialists on particular languages. (It becomes especially obvious when one sees that a considerable number of examples appear in the book with reference to "personal communication" of many people and to comments of "anonymous reviewers" of Cambridge University Press.) I do not know whether the authors plan to realize their Lexicon in a computer database format and to make it accessible on the Web, but that seems to be the best way for a wide circle of linguists interested in grammaticalization phenomena to make their contributions to WLG in an interactive manner. (An example of a similar project which occurs to me is the Universals Archive at Konstanz University, see http://ling.uni-konstanz.de:591/Universals/introduction.html)

    I will conclude with a number of minor critical comments, which treat some points that seem controversial to me or some interpretations of data which I do not agree with. There are quite a few of such comments, but they by no means detract from the merits of this book. I also hope that the notes which follow might be of some use in a new edition.

    1.

    The authors certainly adopt the view that processes of grammatical change usually concern not just lexemes as such, but "more complex conceptual entities, such as phrases, whole propositions or even larger constructions" (p.6). Thus, English "be going to" as a future marker does not have just GO as a source, but rather a construction "GO (progressive aspect) + to + Verb (infinitive form of the verb with purposive meaning)", and similar constructions in other languages also tend to contain auxiliary in imperfective form (usually present tense) and main verb with purposive or allative marking. Reference to "construction" is in a few cases present in WLG already in the entry's heading (like GO TO > ..., COME TO > ..., COME FROM > ...), or it may be commented upon in the text. Nevertheless, there are many cases where this has not been done.

    I agree that it is certainly easier for referential purposes to cite lexeme (e.g. BE, GO, SAY, ONE, etc.) as a name for an entry. However, I think that if a grammatical marker develops from this primary source, at least the following kinds of information should be indicated: (a) what class of words does it modify; (b) what other elements, if any, are involved in the construction; (c) what form does the modified (meaningful) word take -- if it is a part of a verbal periphrastic form, for example, then is it a participle (with some aspectual value: perfective, imperfective, etc.), or a converb (again with characterization of its meaning), or an infinitive, etc.; (d) are there restrictions on the grammatical form of the auxiliary word -- if we take verbal periphrastic forms as an example again, the development of periphrastic forms with future meaning always implies that the auxiliary (like GO, COME, WANT, etc.) is in the present tense.

    Information of (a)-(b) type is always present in WLG, although (c) -- sometimes, and (d) -- almost always -- are absent. Of course, the authors of the book should not be blamed for such cases, as it is obvious that the available sources of information may lack the necessary data or contain vague formulations or inaccuracies; but I think that in general all four parameters (a)-(d) should be included in the format of description (maybe even in the format of the entry's heading).

    2.

    There are some grammaticalization paths which look similar, though this similarity is not commented upon in the text, and sometimes the criteria for differentiating the paths are not quite clear. Thus, there are several paths leading to CONTINUOUS (like IN (spatial) > CONTINUOUS (p.178), LOCATIVE > CONTINUOUS (p.202), COPULA, LOCATIVE > CONTINUOUS (p.97) and EXIST > CONTINUOUS (p.127)), but the corresponding sections do not contain cross-references. Because of this, the similarity of all these paths is obscured, though in fact all the sources listed above are rather realizations of the single Location Schema (mentioned on p.203).

    The paths ABLATIVE > NEAR PAST (p.33-34) and COME FROM > NEAR PAST (p.72-73) are also very close, which is not pointed at in the text. It is also not quite clear why, e.g., French construction "venir de" is treated on p.33 as an example of ABLATIVE -- it rather involves a lexical verb COME that serves as a "marker introducing a spatial participant" (however, on p.73 it is mentioned again under COME FROM > NEAR PAST heading).

    I also wonder if there is really a separate path H-POSSESSIVE ('have') > FUTURE (p.242-243, with famous Romance examples): it is commonly assumed that there is an intermediate meaning of obligation (or, more accurately, "predestination of the object to follow a certain course of events", according to Benveniste (1968: 90)), so there are rather always two successive developments H-POSSESSIVE > OBLIGATION (p.243-245) and OBLIGATION > FUTURE (p.218).

    Although RESULTATIVE ('having reached a new state') is mentioned in the Index of grammatical concepts on p.25, some relevant paths leading from this meaning are not included, like RESULTATIVE > PERFECT or RESULTATIVE > INFERENTIAL (discussed in Bybee et al. (1994), among others). The very source construction common to resultative grams -- "BE/COPULA or HAVE + past participle/converb" -- is also worthy mentioning as a separate entry. I think that in general all (or almost all) developments having COPULA as a source (see pp. 94ff) should be represented as constructions already in the heading.

    3.

    The relationship between an abstract meaning label for a source lexeme and its real meaning in particular language in not always clear. For example, there are cases where several translational equivalents are given for a language-specific lexeme, but one of the meaning seems to be chosen as the "basic" one. Thus, Turkish "dur-" 'stand; wait; remain; endure' is described as a case of REMAIN development (> DURATIVE, p.255), and not as a case of STAND (> CONTINUOUS, p.280), like the Tatar "tor-", for example. The verb "kala" 'to be; exist; remain' from Kongo is treated as an example of EXIST (p.127), and not, say, COPULA or REMAIN.

    I suggest that all meanings of polysemous source items should be added to the Lexicon as separate entries, with a reference to some section where this (language-specific) lexeme is discussed with its "basic" meaning (like 'remain' above for Turkish "dur-").

    By the way, I was also surprised to find Spanish construction with "estar" as durative auxiliary under the entry "STAND > CONTINUOUS" (p.281): I think it is rather a case of development of verb meaning 'to be' from a posture verb (STAND > COPULA) with later evolution of "BE + Verb (imperfective converb)" to continuous aspect marker. The very reason for avoiding the verb BE as a source (in WLG only COPULA and EXIST are found among source labels) is not explained.

    4.

    I was a bit confused with so many paths of development leading to PROXIMATIVE ('be about to'), INTENTION ('intend to'), FUTURE and NEAR FUTURE -- I cannot get rid of the feeling that some of these targets (if not all) may be in fact varieties of one and the same target concept. I also wonder why the authors consistently avoid using the term "prospective", and do not even mention it. This term, introduced by Bernard Comrie (1976: 64-65) as referring to "a present state relative to some future event" (and already containing "seeds of some future situation"), seems to be close, if not identical, to "proximative", described in Heine (1994: 36) as describing "a temporal phase located close to the initial boundary of the situation described by the main verb". Term 'prospective' is used by Suzanne Fleischman, Simon Dik, Oesten Dahl, Michele Emanatian, among others. Bybee et al. (1994) do not mention it either, and the corresponding label they use is 'immediate future'. Anyway, 'prospective' / 'proximative' / 'near future' / 'immediate future', as well as just 'future' (and to some extent also 'intention') are rather different shades of one thing - or, to put it in grammaticalization terms, may be viewed as successive steps on one grammaticalization path, which may look as INTENTION ['the participant intends to do P after the reference point'] > PROSPECTIVE/PROXIMATIVE ['the state of affairs holding at reference point is such that in normal circumstances it will lead to the situation P' -- like in "It's gonna rain"] > (PREDICTIVE) FUTURE ['the speaker predicts that P will happen']. Even if this line of development is not true of all cases, it certainly deserves to be commented upon in WLG. (Note also that the development from referring to the situation (P) connected in some way to the moment of speech (having "present relevance") to referring to the situation without such a connection is parallel to the evolution RESULTATIVE > PERFECT > PAST in the "retrospective" domain.)

    AVERTIVE does not seem to be far from this "family" of meanings, as it is often indistinguishable from 'past prospective/proximative'. Both Comrie and Heine agree that prospective resp. proximative is an aspectual meaning, and if its combination with present tense develops into future, its combination with past is normally interpreted as 'was on the verge of doing' or 'almost did'.

    5.

    The translation of a Russian example on p.94 (from Kuteva (1998)) is not correct: "mashina bylo poexala" does not mean that the car 'nearly started' or 'was just about to start' (which can be expressed as "mashina chut' bylo ne poexala"); it means that the car did start, but - most likely -- almost immediately stopped. That is, we deal here not with the AVERTIVE meaning in Kuteva's sense ('almost; nearly'), but rather with something like "cancelled result" or "interrupted attempt", other meanings from the "antiresultative" family as defined by Plungian (2001) -- 'the result of the action was reached, but it was later cancelled'. By the way, the position of particle "bylo" before the verb seems rather unusual -- although it is quite possible, its postposition (like in "poexala bylo") is much more neutral and frequent.

    6.

    I can't agree with anonymous reader of the book, who notes that there is a reversed direction of the path "HEART (body part) > IN (spatial)" in case of Russian word "serdtse" ('heart') which is derived from a root "sered-" ('middle', like in "sered-ina" 'middle part'; see p.171, footnote). First of all, Russian root "serd-" goes back to the Indo-European level (and corresponds to Latin "cor, cord-", Welsh "cairdd" or English "heart"), so the development, if any, must have taken place in PIE, and not in Russian (see other examples of this root at, e.g., http://indoeuro.bizland.com/project/phonetics/word55.html ). More than that, even if 'heart' and 'middle' have the same root diachronically, what we have here may be a standard derivation of one lexical item from another lexical item (i.e. from a noun or an adverb meaning 'middle' or 'in the middle'), and not a development of a grammatical "marker introducing locative participant" (and that is meant by "IN" label according to p.21) into a noun meaning 'heart'. So, there may be no reversal of grammaticalization process (IN > HEART), but only reversed semantic development ('middle' > 'heart' and 'heart' > 'middle'). Of course, semantic derivation as such can occur without grammaticalization, cf. manifestation of 'heart' > 'middle' in such examples as "in the heart of England" or "heart of oak". In Russian, by the way, there is also a word "serdtsevina" 'core, middle part', which is a derivative from "serdtse" ('heart').

    Amusingly, there is another commentary concerning Russian example, where other directionality is suspected, which also seems strange to me. On p.223 it is suggested that the derivation of an adjective "odinakovyj" ('same') from numeral "odin" ('one') involves "alternative directionality" to the proposed path ONE (numeral) > SAME. I may be missing something, but I think the Russian example provides pure confirmation to this type of change rather that runs contra it.

    7.

    A terminological note: the label "venitive" ('motion hither, 'motion toward'), probably adopted from Bybee et al. (1994), doesn't seem to be correct from the derivational point of view. As far as I understand, terms in "-ive" are derived from Latin supinum/passive participle stems plus suffix "-ive", and not from praesens/infinitive stems plus "-tive": in such cases like "imperative" (Part. imperat-us, Inf. impera-re) it is not so obvious, but cf. permiss-ive < permiss-us (Inf. permitte-re), concess-ive < concess-us (Inf. concede-re), prohibit-ive < prohibit-us (Inf. prohibe-re), ingress-ive < ingress-us (Inf. ingredi) and other cases. "Venitive", conversely, seems to be derived from the infinitive stem "veni-re" ('come') plus "-tive", though the correct variant should be, I guess, "ventive" from supinum stem "vent-um". (I thank Vladimir Plungian for this observation.)

    8.

    On p.78 affix "-kiR-" in Tamil example should be labelled "PRES" instead of "PAST", which is also obvious from the translation. (I thank Anna Smirnitskaya for this observation.)

    9.

    Although the information on language classification given in Appendix 3 is certainly simplified and serves a referential purpose only, it should be better to mention the source of this information (which is probably Ruhlen (1987) mentioned in the list of references; Grimes (2000) and Dalby (2000) are more up-to-date, however). Some notes on subgrouping are not quite consistent (or correct), and I list here those variants that seem to be more felicitous: Ainu = Isolate (not Altaic); Ket = Yenisei Ostyak (not isolate); Tamil = South, Dravidian (like Kannada); Telugu = Telugu-Kui, Dravidian (like Kui); Avar (the same with Lezgian) = Northeast, North, Caucasian (cf. Abaza, Abkhaz, Ubykh, which are labelled "Northwest, North, Caucasian", not just "North, Caucasian"); Surselvan = Romance, Indo-European (or, if necessary, Rhaeto-Romance, Romance, Indo-European).

    10.

    Finally, I think it is desirable to have a language index with page numbers in the book (and maybe also a subject index with page numbers), as readers may be often interested in looking at all the examples of grammaticalization processes listed for their "favourite" language (or at all mentions of a certain term).

    REFERENCES

    Benveniste, Emile (1968) Mutations of linguistic categories // Lehmann, W. P., and Yakov Malkiel, eds. Directions for historical linguistics. Austin & London: University of Texas Press.

    Bybee, Joan L., Revere Perkins, and William Pagliuca (1994) The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Comrie, Bernard (1976) Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Dalby, David (2000) The Linguasphere Register of the World's Languages and Speech Communities. 2 vols. Hebron: Linguasphere Press.

    Grimes, Barbara, ed. (2000) Ethnologue: Languages of the World. 14th edition. S.I.L. [See also http://www.sil.org , http://www.ethnologue.com]

    Heine, Bernd (1994) On the genesis of aspect in African languages: The Proximative // Proceedings of the 20th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: Special Session on historical issues in African linguistics. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistic Society.

    Heine, Bernd, Tom Gueldemann, Christa Kilian-Hatz, Donald A. Lessau, Heinz Roberg, Mathias Schladt, and Thomas Stolz (1993) Conceptual Shift: A Lexicon of Grammaticalization Processes in African Languages. (=Afrikanistische Arbeitspapiere 34-35.) Koeln: Institut fur Afrikanistik.

    Heine, Bernd, and Mechtild Reh (1984) Grammaticalization and reanalysis in African languages. Hamburg: Helmut Buske.

    Kuteva, Tania (1998) On identifying an evasive gram: Action narrowly averted // Studies in Language 22-1: 113-160.

    Lehmann, Christian (1982) Thoughts on Grammaticalization: A Programmatic Sketch. (Arbeiten des Koelner Universalien-Projekts 48). Cologne: Universitaet zu Koeln, Institut fur Sprachwissenschaft.

    Lichtenberk, Frantisek (1991) Semantic change and heterosemy in grammaticalization // Language 67-3: 475-509.

    Maisak, Timur A. (2002) Tipologija grammatikalizacii konstrukcij s glagolami dvizhenija i glagolami pozicii. {= Grammaticalization paths of motion and posture verbs.} Ph.D Dissertation, Moscow State University. [See dissertation abstract in the LINGUIST database.]

    Naess, Ashild (2001) "Eat" as auxiliary // LINGUIST List 12.1631, Wed Jun 20 2001 [See http://www.linguistlist.org/issues/12/12-1631.html].

    Plungian, Vladimir (2001) Antirezul'tativ: do i posle rezul'tata {= Anti-resultative: before and after result} // Plungian, Vladimir, ed. Issledovanija po teorii grammatiki. 1. Glagol'nye kategorii. Moskva.

    Ruhlen, Merritt (1987) A guide to the world's languages, Vol. 1: Classification. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    ABOUT THE REVIEWER

    Timur A. Maisak is a researcher at the Caucasian Languages Dept. of the Institute of Linguistics (Russian Academy of Sciences), Moscow. His main research interests are typology of TMA categories, grammaticalization theory (in particular lexical sources and their possible grammaticalization paths), tense and aspect systems of Nakh-Daghestanian languages, as well as lexical typology of motion verbs. His main publications include chapters describing verbal categories in some minor languages of Daghestan (in part. Bagwalal, Tsakhur, Agul) and papers concerning grammaticalization of verbs denoting motion and posture in the languages of the world. The dissertation "Grammaticalization paths of constructions with motion and posture verbs" was defended at Moscow State University in May, 2002.