LINGUIST List 14.275

Sat Jan 25 2003

Review: Discourse Analysis: Fetzer & Meierkord (2002)

Editor for this issue: Naomi Ogasawara <naomilinguistlist.org>


What follows is a review or discussion note contributed to our Book Discussion Forum. We expect discussions to be informal and interactive; and the author of the book discussed is cordially invited to join in.

If you are interested in leading a book discussion, look for books announced on LINGUIST as "available for review." Then contact Simin Karimi at siminlinguistlist.org.


Directory

  • Chaoqun Xie, Rethinking Sequentiality: Linguistics Meets Conversational Interaction

    Message 1: Rethinking Sequentiality: Linguistics Meets Conversational Interaction

    Date: Sat, 25 Jan 2003 13:16:39 +0000
    From: Chaoqun Xie <cqxie163.com>
    Subject: Rethinking Sequentiality: Linguistics Meets Conversational Interaction


    Fetzer, Anita and Christiane Meierkord, ed. (2002) Rethinking Sequentiality: Linguistics Meets Conversational Interaction. John Benjamins, vi+300pp, hardback ISBN 1-58811-233-0, $87.00, Pragmatics and Beyond New Series 103.

    Announced at http://linguistlist.org/issues/13/13-2821.html

    Chaoqun Xie, Fujian Teachers University

    OVERVIEW

    This collection of papers consisting of two major parts is, as indicated by the two editors Christiane Meierkord and Anita Fetzer in their introduction, an outgrowth of a panel discussion on sequentiality at the 7th IPrA Conference held in Budapest in the year 2000. This volume aims to present a multidisciplinary perspective of both theoretical and applied nature towards the concept of sequentiality, which has received more and more enthusiastic attention from researchers and scholars of convergent or divergent academic backgrounds including ''functional pragmatics, dialogic theory, narrative theory, cognitive pragmatics, relevance theory, psycholinguistics, represented discourse and critical discourse analysis'' (p. 1). In the introduction, the editors begin with a discussion of the four notions of sequence, sequencing, sequential organization and sequentiality. For them, sequence is product-oriented, sequencing is process-oriented, and sequential organization is both; sequencing and sequence are different from sequential organization in that the former can apply to monadic settings while the latter can not. And sequentiality, as a relational concept, is the coherent structuring of sequenced utterances. A relatively comprehensive understanding of these notions might not be possible without the introduction of embeddedness and context. The introduction also examines the notion of sequentiality in the research paradigms of conversation analysis, speech act theory, Gricean pragmatics, dialogue grammar and cognition and closes with a briefing of the chapters that follow.

    Part I is titled ''Sequences in theory and practice: Minimal and unbounded?'' and contains five contributions. The first paper is authored by one of the editors Anita Fetzer. In this paper, Fetzer touches upon communicative intentions in context in a dialogue framework based on the premises of rationality, intentionality, cooperation and ratification. Fetzer begins with an examination of the status of intention in Austin's and Searle's speech act theory, pointing out that a speech act is an essential part of an exchange and depends on the context in which it occurs and that both Austin and Searle do not specify the way in which an interlocutor constructs an utterance in order to perform a specific speech act. Next, Fetzer links communicative intentions to the Gricean cooperative principle, arguing that Fran�ois Recanati's conception of communicative intentions should be introduced into the Gricean cooperative principle to enhance the latter's explanatory power for both social action and cognition. Fetzer then moves on to argue for adapting J�rgen Habermas' Theory of Communication Action to account for the dialogue act of a plus/minus- validity claim anchored to an interactive tripartite system of objective, subjective and social worlds and their presuppositions (p.57), rightly concluding that communicative intentions are interdependent on both social antecedents and social consequences and can no longer be interpreted in the frameworks of single individuals and single speech acts only and that both bottom-up and top-down approaches are needed for conversation investigations. By the way, the first edition of Sperber and Wilson's Relevance came out in 1986 but not in 1996, and the second edition was published in 1995 (See pp. 69, 62; and see also p. 33). And there is also a minor error in the ordering of Levinson's three cited publications (See p. 69).

    The second paper in this part is ''Cognition and narrativity in speech act sequences'' contributed by Marina Sbis�. In this paper, Sbis� calls for thinking once again the cognitive and interactional dynamics underlying the production and comprehension of speech act sequences, showing that sequentiality is a fundamental dimension of speech acts. Specifically, Sbis� argues for two things mainly as clearly stated on page 72: 1. the cognitive component in speech act sequencing is secondary to and dependent on action; 2. one cognitive factor relevant to the understanding of speech act sequences is the so-called ''narrative schemes'', proposed by A. J. Greimas (1983) in the framework of narrative semiotics, that analyzes sequences of events in three main steps: Manipulation, Action and Sanction.

    More specifically, the author first explores speech act sequencing and the production of effects, arguing, among other things, that the production of conventional effects makes speech act sequencing possible only if some kind of by-default agreement has been established between participants about the conventional effect to be produced and that sequencing contributes to the achievement of conventional effects on the part of an illocutionary and to the manifestation of any illocutionary act the interlocutor has performed. This dynamic nature of speech act sequences is then exemplified with a discussion of a sequence from a recorded conversation. After that, the author goes on to claim that ''the speech act sequence as a chain of conventional effects comes into being independently of its cognitive appreciation and that conversational turns do not constitute sequences simply by being produced one after in the same physical circumstances, concluding that we make sense of what happens or is done around us, and of speech act sequences in particular, thanks to the general form provided by the narrative scheme.

    In ''Recurrent sequences and mental processes'', Christiane Meierkord endeavors to expound how the cognitively autonomous individual speaker mentally processes conversational closings. After introducing schemata, script and frame theory, Kintsch's ''construction-integration model'' and striking features attached to conversational closings, the author presents empirical 'thinking-out-loud data' collected in an elicitation task, investigating how these subjects process the discourse marker 'well', prepositional contents, dispreferred utterances and adjacent formulas respectively. Meierkord finds, among other things, that the mental processes during processing and the utterances are both heterogeneous, that different individuals with different experiences perceive utterances in recurring sequences differently, and that more than one script may be at work during the processing of interactional sequences.

    In ''Boundaries and sequences in studying conversation'', Robert B. Arundale and David Good say no to viewing boundaries between conversational units as of vanishingly small duration. For them, boundaries are ex post facto phenomena that emerge in the co- constituting of conversation. Specifically, first, the authors demonstrate that interaction is not monadic but dyadic; second, they introduce the term ''dyadic cognizing'' to develop a model of interactional achievement in conversation complemented with the ''Janus Principle'' which the authors claim plays a fundamental role in our reconceptualization of boundaries and units in conversation, among others. Third, much ink is devoted to the discussion that the boundaries of utterances and of their components must also be co- constituted, where evidence from turn-taking, simultaneous projecting and retroactive assessing, and interdependence in foresight and hindsight are examined. Finally, the authors present some implications their views may bring to bear on conversation analysis, linguistic pragmatics, psycholinguistics and cognition.

    Sara W. Smith and Andreas H. Jucker's chapter presents evidence for the role of intersubjectivity in interactional sequences, with particular emphasis placed on the social and textual contexts of turns consisting of the discourse marker 'well' and on how meaning is mapped onto the turns in interaction. That the authors' effort in taking into consideration the linguistic, social, and cognitive aspects revealed by an investigation into the 'well' turns is quite impressive. For the authors, the 'well' turn may signal the coming of ''dispreferred seconds'', playing an important role in various kinds of interactional sequences under examination: questions, assessments, invitations and advice. The authors conclude that conversationalists may resort to the use of the 'well' turns to negotiate and re-construct their beliefs from time to time for ''keeping the ball rolling'' and that the cognitive abilities on the part of these interlocutors are exploited and displayed in the dynamic interaction.

    Part II under the heading of ''Sequences in discourse: The micro-macro interface'' also contain chapters. In ''Talk on TV: Sequentiality meets intertextuality and interdiscursivity'', Roy Langer shows that the marriage between conversation analysis and critical discourse analysis can shed new light on how we approach sequentiality. Langer starts with comparing and contrasting conversation analysis and critical conversation analysis, arguing that what makes these approaches divergent is the definition of the discourse unit to be analysed and the degree of inclusion of context into the analytical frame (p. 185; cf. Mey, 2001). The author then presents a detailed analysis of two case studies to demonstrate that both intertextuality and interdiscursivity have a decisive influence on the sequential organization of talk on TV.

    In ''Culture, genres and the problem of sequentiality: An attempt to describe local organization and global structures in talk-in- interaction'', Friederike Kern begins with methodological and theoretical preliminaries as regards sequentiality in conversation analysis and job interviews as communicative genres. Then Kern, drawing on a large corpus of 41 authentic job interviews, finds that job candidates under investigation usually list or narrate their professional experience. Further, the author argues that sequential analysis can not always account for the style a speaker adopts in terms of discourse organization and that the participants' linguistic performance is largely determined by genre-specific communicative goals and cultural linguistic practices.

    In ''Argumentative sequencing and its interactional variation'', Thomas Spranz-Fogasy, viewing argumentation as an interactively organized sequence, analyses the contextual implementation and the internal structure of argumentation in particular, showing that the prototypical argumentative sequence follows five steps as follows: triggering, marking dissent or problematization, presenting new explanatory information, acceptance and, ratification. Spranz-Fogasy also touches upon expanded argumentation (by recourse to insertion, serialization) and compressed argumentation, with the conclusion that sequentiality provides for resources for the production of utterances and for mutual reference of the partners with a lot of rhetorical implications.

    In ''Sequential positioning of represented discourse: In institutional media interaction'', Marjut Johansson, resorting to the theoretical framework of dialogism, a constructivist perspective on interaction, adopts ''a multilevel approach'' (from the point of view of adjacency pairs, topic development and cognitive-discursive activity) to examine represented discourse in relation to sequentiality and the function it plays in interaction. Johansson focuses on the analysis of a political interview, in an effort to explore how the parties concerned construct instances of represented discourse in this activity. Johansson shows among other things that both interviewer and interviewee use represented discourse to serve various purposes. On the one hand, both parties use it to state the events; second, the interviewer use represented discourse as an intertextual device which the politician use it as a rhetorical device in counter-argumentation and in explanation with the aim of creating a position.

    Finally, in ''interactional coherence in discussion and everyday storytelling: On considering the role of jedenfalls and auf jeden fall'', Kristin B�hrig examines the German expressions 'auf jeden fall' and 'jedenfalls' in everyday storytelling and academic discourse, showing that they affect a synchronization of the interlocutors' linguistic and mental activities that had strayed in the different directions during the conversation. By the way, the word 'been' in ''... which can been seen as a result of sequentiality'' (p. 273) should be 'be'.

    CRITICAL EVALUATION

    As can be seen from the above relatively detailed description, this volume has shown that the form and function of a sequence embedded in interaction can be variable with many factors at work such as context, cognition and knowledge structure of interlocutors. In other words, contextualization and dynamics of sequentiality should not be put aside when it comes to accounting for how interlocutors act in interaction to achieve various goals. This collection of papers has made an admirable endeavor to provide a both varied and unified account of the very notion of sequentiality in the sense that different methodological considerations can contribute to our deeper understanding of what can be revealed by investigating sequences in interaction, making great headway in unraveling the mysteries of sequentiality and various aspects in close connection with talk-in-interaction, cognitive, linguistic, pragmatic and cultural, among other things.

    And I have some questions as follows. The editors claim that sequential organization ''refers to the joint construction of one or more sequences and is thus intrinsically linked to the turn-taking mechanism'' (p. 4) and that it ''requires a dyadic or multi-party setting'' (p. 4). I was wondering if it is always the case; for me, sequential organization can also find expression in a monadic setting. The other day I watched a Chinese entertainment program on TV where a female comp�re presents some background information before and during the program is going on. >From her presentation one may track a certain kind of sequential organization, the discussion of which goes, of course, beyond the scope of the present review. In point of fact, as noted by Mey (2001: 614), much of present-day conversation analysis has been focused on and limited to ''English or another European language: German, French, Spanish, or one of the Scandinavian languages.'' This is also true of the present volume under review. In this sense, more research on conversation analysis in other languages should be aimed for.

    My next question is about the areas covered in this volume. As I see it, that this volume does not touch upon the relationship between gender and sequentiality is painfully obvious. I am interested in what impact gender differences might have on sequential organization and the notion of sequentiality. Actually, what contributions conversation analysis can make to gender studies has not received enough attention. Recently, Weatherall (2002), for instance, adopts a conversation- analytic approach to account for ''the interactional mechanisms underling the omnirelevance of gender in daily life''. Another area awaiting much more concern is the investigation of the impact of power on sequential organization. Recent studies (e. g. Itakura 2001; Thornborrow 2002) show that power is not pre-patterned or static but, more often than not, measurable and contextually sensitive, which would greatly affect the sequences in interactions. Further efforts should be oriented to this exploration.

    To sum up, given the interdisciplinary or multidisciplinary nature of sequentiality in talk-in-interaction, this volume should be of much value and great interest to many people, those doing conversation analysis and discourse analysis in particular.

    REFERENCES

    Itakura, Hiroko. 2001. Describing conversational dominance. Journal of Pragmatics 33: 1859-1880.

    Mey, Inger. 2001. The CA/CDA controversy. Journal of Pragmatics 33: 609-615.

    Sperber, Dan, Wilson, Deirdre. 1986/1995. Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Thornborrow, Joanna. 2002. Power talk: Language and interaction in institutional discourse. Harlow, England: Pearson Education.

    Weatherall, Ann. 2002. Towards understanding gender and talk-in- interaction. Discourse & Society 13(6):767-781.

    ABOUT THE REVIEWER

    Chaoqun Xie is a lecturer with the Foreign Languages Institute, Fujian Teachers University in Fuzhou, China. His main areas of research interests include pragmatics, sociolinguistics, culture, communication and translation.