LINGUIST List 5.1088
Thu 06 Oct 1994
Disc: French clitics
Editor for this issue: <>
Directory
Philip H. Miller, On the affixal status of French clitic pronouns
Lambrecht Knut, Re: 5.1074 French clitics
Message 1: On the affixal status of French clitic pronouns
Date: Wed, 5 Oct 1994 09:12:50 +On the affixal status of French clitic pronouns
From: Philip H. Miller <pmillerulb.ac.be>
Subject: On the affixal status of French clitic pronouns
This posting presents a joint response by Julie Auger and Philip
Miller to the recent discussion on LINGUIST concerning the status of
clitic pronouns in French, specifically, the postings by Steven
Schaufele and Pierre Larrivee.
In his summary (LINGUIST 5.1036) of the responses he got
concerning topicalization and clitic doubling in French and Italian,
Schaufele notes that various linguists have suggested that clitic
pronouns are being reanalyzed in French as prefixed agreement
morphemes.
Larrivee (LINGUIST 5.1074) suggests that this analysis is problematic
on the basis of various phenomena, concerning (i) the possible
morphological complexity of clitic pronouns; (ii) the mobility of clitic
pronouns; (iii) the fact that subject clitics are restricted to subject
function.
There has been an extensive debate on the status of pronominal
clitics in French (and the other Romance languages) over the past 25
years. This debate has brought to bear on the question a huge
amount of complex data which cannot be ignored in this context.
Both authors of this posting have independently concluded from
these data that pronominal clitics in French should be analyzed as
lexically attached inflectional affixes (see references listed below).
A first point that we would like to make, specifically addressing
Schaufele's post, is that it is necessary to distinguish affixal status
from agreement marker status. It is clearly possible for a language to
have affixal pronominals ("incorporated pronouns") which are not
agreement markers. This is the case, for instance, in many Bantu
languages where the object marker, though it is clearly word internal,
can be incompatible with a clause internal overt object NP (cf. e.g.
Chichewa (Bresnan and Mchombo 1987), and also the discussion by
Benji Wald in a recent response on LINGUIST 5.968 to a query by
Dan Everett). Consequently, the question of whether the clitic
pronouns in French are lexically attached or not is independent of the
question of whether they are agreement markers. The complex data
concerning clitic doubling are only relevant to the latter question, not
the former (obligatoriness or optionality with various types of overt
NPs, definite, indefinite, pronominal, etc. and depending on the
syntactic function of the NP; prosodic properties of the doubling overt
NP; locality constraints between the clitic bearing verb and the
doubling overt NP; etc.). Julie Auger has argued in detail that subject
clitics in spoken Quebec French are being reanalyzed as agreement
markers, though not the other clitics. The doubling properties in
Quebec French clearly indicate that reanalysis of object clitics as
agreement markers has not (yet) taken place.
The second question then is the issue of lexically attached
inflectional status vs. independent word status. Note that under the
latter hypothesis, French pronominal clitics should be analyzed as
postlexical clitics, i.e. prosodically deficient words which are
prosodically attached to a neighboring host word.
The references below provide numerous types of data arguing in
favor of lexically attached inflectional status. Here is a very brief
summary:
(i) Morphophonological idiosyncrasies, which cannot be accounted
for on the basis of productive phonological rules, in the realization of
the clitics and, more remarkably, in the realization of the stem to
which they are attached (e.g. the pronunciation of 'je suis' (I am) as
'chuis' in France and 'chu' in Quebec, both of which are impossible
for the phonetically identical 'je suis' form of the verb 'suivre' (to
follow).
(ii) Gaps in the paradigms, where there is no grammatical form with
clitics. In some cases 'strong' pronouns can be used instead in the
position of the usual overt NP argument, or one has to resort to a
completely different construction. (E.g. '*Marie me te/lui decrit.' vs.
'Marie me decrit a toi/elle' (Marie describes me to you/her);
'*Chante-je?' vs. 'Est-ce que je chante?', or 'Je chante?', or 'Je
chante-tu?', (the last, derived from 'je chante-ti?', exists only in
Quebec French) (Am I singing?)). Note that it is hard to see how these
gaps could be explained in syntactic terms, especially for the cases
where a strong pronoun alternative is possible. Similarly, the
existence of well formed alternatives to the impossible clitic clusters
shows that the problems are neither semantic nor pragmatic.
(iii) Complex properties of the ordering and compatibility of the clitics
among themselves, for which no satisfactory syntactic analysis has
been proposed to date; the plausibility of the existence of a non ad
hoc syntactic analysis is greatly cast into doubt by the variation in
clitic ordering attested between otherwise extremely similar dialects
(cf. e.g. Cummins and Roberge 1994 on this).
(iv) The fact that pronominal clitics must appear attached to a Verb,
to the exclusion of any other category (note that this cannot be
explained as a consequence of VP initial attachment, since in those
-cases where there is a preverbal element in the VP, the clitics attach
to the verb in modern French ('Il faut ne rien lui dire' vs. '*Il faut ne lui
rien dire'). Usually, items which are argued to be postlexical clitics do
not impose such constraints on the category of the word to which they
are prosodically attached.
(v) The fact that pronominal object clitics in French cannot have
scope over a coordination of hosts ('*Marie le voit et entend' vs.
'Marie le voit et l'entend' (Marie sees and hears her.)). In fact, we
have argued that even for subject clitics, wide scope over
coordination is abnormal in colloquial French ('%Je mange du pain
et/pis bois du vin. 'I eat bread and drink wine').
(vi) The types of phonological rules affecting clitic+host
combinations. It appears that there are lexical phonological rules
which apply between clitics and host (e.g. that liaison phenomena are
of a word internal type, rather than of a postlexical type). These data
are among the least reliable of those presented here.
(For the relevance of these tests, see among others Zwicky and
Pullum 1983).
Let us reconsider the data proposed by Larrivee in this context.
>Take third person
>nominative clitik "il". It has its own morphology (number (vs Ils),
gender
>(vs Elle) and case (vs Le (accusative) and Lui (dative?)).
The only case where a productive morphological relationship between
these forms can be suggested is the case of the singular/plural
alternation which shares the orthographic -s suffix with the regular
sg/pl alternation for nominals. This could just as well be analyzed as
a frozen remnant of previously productive morphology, all the more
so that other forms show irregular alternations for sg/pl. This is the
case for all 1st and second person forms (e.g. je,me/on, tu,te/vous, ...)
and for the dative and reflexive forms of the 3rd person (lui/leur, se/se).
> It can move around (Il le dit/Le dit-il?).
To which we can add postverbal object clitics in imperatives ('Dis-le!'
vs. '*Le dis!' (Say it!)).
This in fact is one of the most convincing arguments against the
affixal analysis we propose. However there a number of
counterarguments.
a) The form of certain clitics changes depending on whether they are pre
or postverbal. ('Tu me regardes.' vs. 'Regarde-moi', similarly for
te/toi, and, in many varieties of French, for 'y' and 'en' which are
pronounced [zi] and [za~] postverbally 'Donne-moi-z-en'). Moreover,
the pre- and postverbal forms are not related by productive phonological
rules: if we assume 'me -> moi' under accent by such a rule, we would
predict that 'le -> loi' in similar cases, which is not the case
('Prend-le!' vs. '*Prend-loi!' (Take it!)).
b) The ordering of clitics before and after the verb is different ('Marie
me le donnera.' 'Donne-le-moi!' (Marie will give it to me.; Give it to
me!)
c) As noted above, for most verbs, subject clitic inversion is
impossible with 'je' ('*Chante-je?' (Am I singing?) vs. 'Puis-je?' (May
I?)), which is hard to understand if 'je' is an independent word:
syntactic fronting of the verb should apply independently of person.
There is nothing phonologically illformed about the resulting sequence
that would explain how it is filtered out by prosodic cliticization. If
these forms are produced by the morphology, this is simply a
case of gaps in a paradigm. Note that the restrictions on subject clitic
inversion are much stronger in Quebec Colloquial French: only the
2nd person pronouns can be inverted.
d) There are a number of languages with what have been called
'mobile position affixes' which have been documented. I.e. it does not
appear to be crosslinguistically impossible for an affix to appear as a
prefix in certain forms, but as a suffix in others. (cf. e.g. Fulmer 1991,
Noyer in press).
Finally, we fail to understand the relevance of the third argument ("it
['il'] is restricted to subject function")
Julie Auger Dept. of Linguistics, McGill University
injomusicb.mcgill.ca
Philip Miller English and General linguistics, Universit de Lille 3
pmillerulb.ac.be
Auger, Julie. 1993. "More evidence for verbal agreement-marking in
Colloquial French". In W. J. Ashby et al. (eds.), Linguis-
tic Perspectives on the Romance Languages. Amsterdam:
Benjamins, pp. 177-198.
Auger, Julie. 1994. "Subject-clitic inversion in Romance: A morpho-
logical analysis". Paper presented at LSRL 24, Los Angeles.
Auger, Julie. 1994. Pronominal Clitics in Quebec Colloquial French:
A Morphological Analysis. Ph.D. dissertation, University
of Pennsylvania.
Auger, Julie. in press. "Les clitiques pronominaux en francais parle
informel: une approche morphologique". To appear in Revue
quebecoise de linguistique.
Cummins, Sarah & Yves Roberge. 1994. "A morphosyntactic
analysis of Romance clitic constructions". In M. Mazzola
(ed.), Issues and Theory in Romance Linguistics. Washing-
ton, D.C.: Georgetown University Press, pp. 239-257.
Fulmer, S. Lee. 1991. "Dual-position affixes in Afar:
an argument for phonologically-drivent morphology". In
A. I. Halpern (ed.), The Proceedings of the Ninth WCCFL
Stanford: CSLI, pp. 189-203.
Miller, Philip. 1992. Clitics and Constituents in Phrase Structure
Grammar. New York, Garland (Doctoral Dissertation, University of
Utrecht, 1991).
Miller, Philip H. 1992. "Postlexical cliticization vs. affixation:
Coordination criteria". Proceedings of the 28th meeting of the
Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 382-396.
Miller, Philip H. and Ivan A. Sag. to appear. "French clitic
movement without clitics or movement". To appear in Revue
quebecoise de linguistique.
Noyer, Rolf. in press. "Mobile affixes in Huave: optimality
and morphological well-formedness". To appear in E. Duncan
et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the Twelfth WCCFL. Stanford:
CSLI.
Message 2: Re: 5.1074 French clitics
Date: Tue, 4 Oct 1994 12:11:42 -Re: 5.1074 French clitics
From: Lambrecht Knut <lambrechspot.Colorado.EDU>
Subject: Re: 5.1074 French clitics
The recent posting by Pierre Larrivee about French clitics reminds me of an
exchange on the Linguist some time ago, about the same topic, triggered (as
far as I remember) by a contribution by Julie Auger. I don't think it'll be
very useful to start another debate on the century-old question of whether
French "clitics" are pronouns or "morphemes" (to use Pierre Larrivee's term),
and I have tried to justify my skepticism in a reply to the posting by Julie
Auger.
But I can't help reacting to the following remark by Pierre Larrivee:
> There is one further cost to this analysis. Dislocations in
> general can be put pretty much anywhere in a sentence ("(Les enfants) je les
> ai vus (les enfants) qui foutaient le camp (les enfants)"); so French would
> then not only be pro-drop but also non-configurational!!! I invite linguists
> to ponder seriously on such a proposal, and i hope those few elements here
> will help them make an enlightened choice.
Independently of the issue of whether LES in the above example is a
morpheme/inflection marker/clitic/bound pronoun or not, I take issue
with the claim that detached (dislocated) NPs can go "pretty much anywhere
in a sentence". I have discussed the issue of the syntactic status of left
and right detached constituents in some detail in my 1981 monograph 'Topic,
antitopic, and verb agreement in non-standard French, and a little more
rigorously in my 1986 UC Berkeley dissertation, and I think I have shown
that the position of such constituents is severely constrained, in a way
that is exactly what we would expect from topic constituents.
Essentially, LEFT-detached NPs are outside of (what used to be called) S',
i.e. they precede the unit [COMP - S] (i.e. the TOP position of Chomsky
197(?), "On WH-movement", sorry I forgot the exact reference). They have
severe restrictions on embedding, as shown e.g. in
*Les films que Pierre il aime le plus ont tous ete interdits.
with variation depending on the degree to which the propositional content
of the embedded clause is asserted or presupposed, and with a certain
subject-object asymmetry ("subject" dislocated NPs are easier to embed than
"object" ones). (This is of course water on the mill of those who want to
see detached NPs as "regular subjects".) So while in the relative clause
island above the detachment is very bad, it's less bad, and sometimes
perfectly ok, in complement clauses, such as
Je crois que les enfants ils ont encore casse de la vaisselle.
but less good when the NP corefers with an object pronoun, as in
?Je crois que la vaisselle les enfants ne l'ont pas encore faite.
and positively awful in
?? Avant que la vaisselle je la fasse je veux finir de manger.
More interesting perhaps is the case of RIGHT-detached constituents,
such as the two occurrences in Pierre Larrivee's example sentence. These
constituents are chomsky-adjoined to the minimal S, and as a result they
have no restriction whatsoever on embedding:
Les films dont je lui ai parle, a Pierre, (ils) ont tous ete interdits.
and they show no subject-object asymmetry. The right-detached NP (I call it
"antitopic NP") is always POSTFOCAL , i.e. it must follow the focus accent
in the clause, which typically coincides with the last constituent of S, so
we get:
Quand est-ce que tu as vu les enfants?
Je les ai vi HIER, les enfants (ok)
# Je les ai vus les enfants HIER (bad)
Qu'est-ce qu'ils ont fait les enfants?
Ils ont casse de la VAISSELLE, les enfants. (ok)
*Ils ont casse les enfants de la VAISSELLE. (real bad)
This is so because detached constituents are topics, hence outside the
focus domain, which in French is the minimal clause.
There is one apparent exception: right-detached NPs can precede direct
object complements as long as these are clausal, even if the clausal
complement is focal:
Je lui ai DIT, a Pierre, que je n'avais pas envie de le VOIR.
but not:
*Je lui ai dit a Pierre quelques MOTS.
This is reminiscent of the well-known fact that locality constraints are
different between V+NP and V+S', as observed also in English (see e.g. Peter
Sells' "Lectures", somewhere on "adjacency"). Nevertheless it's an
exception to my nice simple position rule, and it bothers me. (Anyone
out there who can help me to be less bothered?)
Pierre Larrivee's relative-clause example is NOT a counterexample. The
example he gives (Je les ai vus les enfants qui foutaient le camp) does not
involve a complex NP relative but a perception-verb relative construction,
in which the relative clause is a sister (sic) to the "main" clause (see
the section on French relative clauses in my dissertation), hence the
right-detached NP "les enfants" is in its expected post-clausal and
post-focal position.
Enough already. There is much more to say on these constructions, but the
above may give an impression. I hope to present a coherent argument on all
this soon in a book I'm working on ("The syntax of pragmatics in spoken
French", mmmmhhh!).
The moral is: detached NPs are extra-clausal or extra-sentential, except
in very specific cases, where the NP bound to a subject pronoun does behave
more or less like a subject NP. (But not in all respects! see my earlier
posting in reply to Julie Auger).
Knud Lambrecht
(UT Austin, presently CU Boulder)