AUTHOR: Siemund, Peter TITLE: Pronominal Gender in English SUBTITLE: A Study of English Varieties from a Cross-Linguistic Perspective SERIES TITLE: Routledge Studies in Germanic Linguistics PUBLISHER: Routledge (Taylor and Francis) YEAR: 2008
Peter de Swart, Center for Language and Cognition Groningen, University of Groningen (the Netherlands)
SUMMARY This book presents a revised version of the author's postdoctoral dissertation (Habilitationsschrift, Freie Universität Berlin). It provides an in-depth study of pronominal gender in English with particular focus on the use of animate pronouns (he, she) for inanimate referents. This is done by investigating the distribution of pronominal forms in different varieties of English which in turn are put in a wider cross-linguistic perspective. The goal of the book is to show that this distribution is subject to essentially the same principles in all varieties of English. The study assumes a variationist approach and combines it with a functional-typological perspective. At the same time it touches on various other disciplines in linguistics, such as sociolinguistics and historical linguistics.
Chapter 1-Introduction The introductory chapter presents the roadmap to the book introducing the problem, major claims, and background information. First, it exemplifies and formulates the problem central to the study: the use of animate pronouns (he, she) for inanimate referents and vice versa (i.e. the use of 'it' for animate referents) in different varieties of English. It is indicated that the book is basically concerned with the former alternation. Its main objective is to explain the use of 'he/she' instead of 'it' and to a lesser extent the distribution of masculine and feminine pronouns across different domains of the inanimate world. The basic claim introduced by the author is that pronominal gender in English basically depends on the degree of individuation of the referent. When individuation is conceptualized as a hierarchy or continuum, he predicts that animate pronouns enter the hierarchy from the left whereas inanimates do so from the right. Moreover, it is predicted that most cut-off points on the hierarchy are attested. The chapter continues with an illustration of the phenomenon under discussion from other Germanic and Romance languages, some discussion of the status of the category of gender in English (the author claims that there is no conclusive evidence for the existence of such a category and therefore speaks of pronominal gender), and a note on the historical development of the English gender system which shows that the language developed a semantic gender system from a morphophonological one and that pronominal reference related to semantic gender existed already in Old English times. The next two sections introduce the sources for the variation data used in the study and the generalizations about the distribution of pronominal forms which have been based on them. In the final section the author discusses how his variationist approach ties in with the methodology of functional typology. He claims that his study shows several commonalities with traditional typological studies. Both systematically compare one specific grammatical domain across different varieties/languages and they share the assumption that the observed variation is systematic. Finally, he argues that the study of varieties of a language can make an important contribution to typology as it makes the empirical basis of typological studies more complete.
In chapters 2 to 6 the author presents the data and distribution of pronominal forms in different varieties of English.
Chapter 2-The Southwest of England This chapter discusses pronominal usage in the southwest of England, in particular the variety spoken in West Somerset. The choice for this dialect area is motivated by the amount of information available, in the words of the author ''it is the single most profoundly investigated dialect area of Great Britain'' (p.19). Most importantly, this variety has been argued to draw a clear-cut distinction between count nouns and mass nouns, using animate for the former and inanimates for the latter. The author shows that a similar pattern can be found throughout Great Britain in this way becoming ''a characteristic of all, or at least most, dialects of Great Britain'' (p.23). However, West Somerset English has generalized the usage to the greatest extent and has the most data available therefore becoming the focus of the remainder of this chapter. First the author provides some background information on West Somerset English grammar through the discussion of a number of phenomena related to the mass/count distinction and the pronominal system. Section 2.2 presents an in-depth study of pronominal usage in 19th-century West Somerset English on the basis of approximately 1000 pronominal references from Elworthy's (1886) _West Somerset Word Book and Glossary_. These data show that the proposed generalization does not hold categorically. One finds both masculine pronouns replacing mass nouns and neuter pronouns replacing count nouns. The former exceptions are only found with concrete nouns showing that the mass/count distinction does not hold for abstract nouns which are always taken up by 'it'. Support for count agreement of animate pronouns is also found in a study of poetry texts although some differences emerge as well: the neuter pronoun is used both for count and mass nouns and the author finds use of animate pronouns for abstract nouns. In the final part of the chapter the author compares his findings from 19th-century West Somerset English with pronominal usage in modern (20th Century) English of the Southwest. The conclusion is that the pattern found in West Somerset extends across the southwest of England although the system is in serious decline. Data from the British National Corpus even seem to suggest that the traditional southwestern system has become extinct.
Chapter 3-Newfoundland In this chapter the author provides a short discussion of pronominal usage in the variety of English spoken in Newfoundland, Canada. The system of Newfoundland English shows parallels with that of Southwest England which can probably be traced back to the fact that many settlers came from that area and exported their system of pronominal gender. In contrast to the southwest of England, Newfoundland English still uses this traditional system nowadays. On the basis of two previous studies, the author shows that this variety follows the pattern found in West Somerset English: animate pronouns are used for concrete countable nouns and the neuter pronoun for mass nouns and abstract nouns. He finds some examples of the masculine pronoun being used for abstract nouns but they involve only a handful of count nouns (e.g. 'poem', 'song') making this a phenomenon of restricted scope. The one point in which Newfoundland English differs substantially from West Somerset English is the existence of an additional split in the category of inanimate concrete count nouns. Within this category a distinction is made between nouns that are mobile ('boat', 'car', 'areoplane') which are referred to with the feminine pronoun and ones that are non-mobile ('paddle', 'wheel', 'wing') which are referred to with the masculine pronoun. The author discusses two hypotheses as to the origin of this additional distinction: (i) it is a remnant of the imported variety from Southwest England, as argued by Paddock (1991); (ii) it is a development of Newfoundland English due to contact with other English dialects spoken in Newfoundland, as argued by Wagner (2004). He favors the second proposal as there does not seem to be any evidence for the first in the traditional dialects from Southwest England, but there is evidence that in North-American English feminine pronouns can be used for all sorts of things including moving objects.
Chapter 4-Tasmania and other parts of Australia The use of animate, in particular feminine, pronouns for inanimate referents in varieties of Australian English is mentioned in many dictionaries and handbooks of (Australian) English. There is, however, an enormous scarcity of systematic studies of this phenomenon. The one exception is Tasmanian English, a variety which is also different in morphophonological and other syntactic features with respect to (standard) Australian English. In this chapter, the author embarks on a systematic study of pronominal usage in this variety of Australian English on the basis of recordings of natural and spontaneous conversations made by Andrew Pawley. He shows that the Tasmanian system parallels that found in Southwest England and Newfoundland in the pronominalization of inanimate count nouns by animate pronouns. He observes a strong cline from concrete count nouns to abstract nouns such that nouns with concrete reference are considerably more likely to be pronominalized by animate pronouns than those with abstract reference. The difference with the previously discussed varieties is that Tasmanian Vernacular English makes extensive use of feminine forms instead of masculine ones. The author shows that the semantic domains in which masculine pronouns are used are extremely restricted, including animals of male and unknown sex, plants and trees. Furthermore, some semantic domains (e.g. food and drinks) show optionality of feminine versus neuter pronoun use. Discussion of data from other regions of Australian English mainly support the generalizations made for Tasmanian Vernacular English.
Chapter 5-Informal spoken American English This chapter presents an excursion into pronominal usage in some informal spoken varieties of American English. It aims to show that the fundamental claim of the book (i.e. pronominal gender depends on the degree of individuation of the referent) has relevance above and beyond specific regional varieties of English. This is done by re-evaluating the data from a sociolinguistic study by Matthiot (1979), who collected data in the Los Angeles area and in Buffalo. The sociolinguistic hypothesis tested by Mathiot was whether the use of 'he' and 'she' reflects the (emotional) attitude of the speaker. She assumes that the use of animate pronouns for inanimate nouns is tantamount to the upgrading or assimilation of a non-human entity to a human being. Moreover, she concludes that there are probably no restrictions on the kinds of non-human entities that allow upgrading. The author of the present study aims to show that the data allow for a reinterpretation in terms of individuation of the referent. He shows that indeed animate pronouns are mainly used for individuated inanimate nouns. Furthermore, a grouping of the data in terms of semantic domains shows that some (e.g. animals of unknown sex) are regularly masculine, others are feminine (e.g. environment), and that for some domains (e.g. tools and instruments) pronoun choice depends on the sex of the speaker. Thus, the author concludes that the usage of pronominal gender in informal spoken American English is more systematic than previously assumed.
Chapter 6-Fictional texts This chapter presents data on pronominal usage from written texts making use of the approximately 300 examples of animate pronouns used for inanimate referents in a study by Svartengren (1927). The data consist of created examples of informal speech from novels written in the English language. The examples only contain feminine pronouns to the exclusion of masculine ones and in certain cases the animated usage may be analyzed as a stylistic effect. Notwithstanding these differences with the earlier discussed spoken data, the author wants to show in this chapter that the data from fictional texts by and large align with the data from the other varieties. Where Svartengren proposes that the use of animate pronouns for inanimate referents signals a special attitude of the speaker towards those referents, he also raises the suspicion that individuation might play an important role. In order to test this hypothesis the author analyzes almost all examples discussed by Svartengren which contain mostly count nouns, some mass nouns, and a considerable amount of abstract nouns making the ratio of animate pronouns with reference to mass and abstract concepts much higher in fictional texts than in the naturally occurring data. Nevertheless, the author concludes that despite these differences the general trend found in the other varieties is also present in fictional texts: animate pronouns pick out inanimate referents ranking high in terms of individuation.
Chapter 7-Generalizations across varieties of English The goal of Chapter 7 is to pull together the commonalities linking pronominal gender across different varieties of English. It starts with a recapitulation of pronoun usage in the regional varieties of Southwest England, Newfoundland, and Tasmania, which shows that all three use animate pronouns for concrete count nouns with possible further distinctions within the category of inanimates, and the neuter pronoun for mass or abstract nouns. In the second section the author presents the following hierarchy or continuum of individuation as ''a useful aid furnishing our understanding of these systems'' (p.140): proper names > humans > animals > inanimate tangible objects > abstracts > mass nouns. The systems of the regional varieties discussed in the book can be directly mapped onto this hierarchy and each establishes a different cut-off point. The author argues that the use of this hierarchy has several advantages. It can be used to assess the degree in which regional varieties differ from standard English through comparison of cut-off points. It has predictive power concerning general distributional patterns: due to the Semantic Map Connectivity Hypothesis only systems with a contiguous or connected area in the hierarchy are expected to occur and those that divide the semantic space underlying the hierarchy in more than two parts are ruled out. Finally, the hierarchy is an explanatory instrument which has proven its usefulness in several other domains of grammar in this way raising the problem of pronominal gender to a more general and abstract level. At the same time, the author also voices some concerns in using the hierarchy as an explanation. First, the hierarchy is an inductive generalization and therefore an explanation in terms of it 'must perforce remain relatively weak' (p. 143). Second, there does not seem to be a motivation for the relationship between grammatical marking and the hierarchy of individuation. Here the author concurs that ''if the hierarchy of individuation could convincingly be shown to have basis in human perception or cognition in general, which I believe it has, this would certainly increase the possibility of it being used as an explanatory tool for the analysis of grammatical distinctions. Nevertheless, even if such an analogy can be established, it does not necessarily follow that grammatical marking obeys the hierarchy of individuation just because human cognition is based on this hierarchy'' (p. 144). The chapter closes with a section arguing that pronominal usage in informal spoken American English as well as in fictional texts is in harmony with the hierarchy of individuation.
Chapter 8-Modern Standard English In general the system of pronominal gender in Modern Standard English has a clear semantic basis involving animacy and sex of the referent. Nevertheless, there's also a certain amount of variability and this chapter is concerned with those nouns for which variation in pronoun usage is possible. The chapter starts off with a discussion of the relationship between specific and generic reference and high versus low degree in individuation. In section 8.1 the author discusses and rejects the postulation of a category of gender in English. After a short overview of nouns for which the use of gendered pronouns is invariable (e.g. 'man', 'woman', 'king', 'queen'), a description of nouns for which variable pronoun usage is possible follows. Within the class of human referring nouns we find the so-called personal dual gender nouns (e.g. 'parent', 'friend') which allow both masculine and feminine reference, common gender nouns (e.g. 'child', 'baby') which allow anaphoric reference with animate and neuter pronouns, and collective nouns for which the neuter pronoun or plural 'they' can be used. The other two classes are nouns referring to animals and inanimates referring nouns which are the central concern of the book. The final section of the chapter presents a review of proposed factors influencing pronoun usage, including personification and sympathy of the speaker, and through the discussion of two empirical studies aims to show that the variation in pronoun usage is determined by a few well-defined constraints, with special reference to individuation. The first study by MacKay and Konishi (1980) investigates the use of animate and inanimate pronouns for non-human antecedents in children's literature. They examine the correlation of pronoun use with six variables some of which (specificity, centrality) seem to be related to the notion of individuation as advanced by the author. He concludes that the variation between animate and inanimate pronouns ''is more complex than is usually assumed to be the case and goes beyond a simple contrast in terms of animacy'' (p. 169). The second study reported is by Newman and contrasts the use of 'he/she' with that of 'they'. It shows that the choice between the two options is not only related to the wish to maintain sex-neutrality but also to individuation. It shows that the singular pronouns pattern with referents high in individuation, whereas 'they' patterns with those low in individuation. The conclusion the author draws from the discussion in this chapter is that although the pronominal gender system of Modern Standard English is based on relatively simple semantic principles, it also has its intricacies.
Chapter 9-A Cross-Linguistic View on English Varieties This chapter puts the use of pronominal gender in English in a wider cross-linguistic perspective. It contains two sections on Germanic languages, one on Romance languages, and a third one on other languages. The scope of the study is all agreement triggered by a nominal head that involves contrasts which can be related to the hierarchy of individuation. In the first section on Germanic the author presents data from varieties of Germanic which exhibit a system of pronominal gender based on the mass/count distinction comparable to the ones reported for English. The dialects and languages discussed include the Danish dialect spoken in West Jutland, the variety of Frisian spoken on Helgoland, Dutch, and Afrikaans. The next section shows that in many Germanic languages and dialects the assignment of a noun to a particular gender class can be partially motivated by the mass/count distinction and the hierarchy of individuation. The section on Romance languages shows that central Italian dialects make a distinction within the non-feminine gender between count (masculine) and mass (neuter) nouns which is signaled on articles, demonstratives, pronouns, and adjectives. Likewise, in Asturian Spanish there is mass/count agreement on pronouns and adjectives. The final section of the chapter presents data from a wide variety of languages illustrating gender agreement influenced by the degree of individuation of the noun (e.g. in Arabic and Bantu), the correlation between gender and the mass/count distinction (e.g. in Bantu and Daghestanian), and gender systems organized on the basis of the animate/inanimate distinction (e.g. Algonquian and Niger-Congo).
Chapter 10-The Categorial Status of Pronominal Mass/Count Agreement The aim of this chapter is to contribute to the correct categorization of agreement systems based on the mass/count distinction. Previously these have been analyzed in terms of gender or number but the author wants to argue that they possess certain aspectualizing functions as well. He first points out that a conception of the mass/count distinction in terms of a binary opposition associated with different nominal entries in the lexicon cannot hold true. Instead, the mass/count distinction seems to be a property of noun occurrences. Mass/count agreement systems are often interpreted in terms of gender, the author argues that such an analysis is problematic for several reasons including the fact that mass/count gender systems are rare cross-linguistically and only involve pronouns whose deictic use is not compatible with an agreement category like gender, but most importantly the fact that the mass/count distinction is not a lexical property of nouns. A similar story holds for the interpretation of the systems as number systems. From this discussion, the author concludes that ''the mass/count agreement systems at issue here are more likely to be analyzed as number systems than as gender systems'' (p. 236). Finally, the author puts forward an analysis of the mass/count agreement systems in terms of nominal aspect. This is inspired by the fact that speakers can use pronominal agreement to portray an entity as individuated or non-individuated. This is reminiscent of verbal aspect which can be used to express different perspectives on a situation. Moreover, there is a close semantic relationship between verbal aspect and the mass/count distinction. Nevertheless interpretation of these mass/count agreement systems in terms of nominal aspect remains questionable on both formal and distributional grounds. The conclusion of this chapter must then be that 'we cannot but admit that the mass/count systems discussed in this study apparently have some properties of gender systems, number systems and perhaps even aspectual systems' (p. 241).
Chapter 11-Conclusion and Outlook The final chapter of this study summarizes the previous chapters and presents some open issues and loose ends including historical, sociolinguistic and theoretical problems.
EVALUATION Siemund's book is a well-written and clearly structured study. With its accessible style and explanatory clarity the book is extremely reader friendly. Throughout the book the author displays an impressive patience in his descriptions of previous research and the discussion of the data, and almost nowhere does he rush to premature conclusions. Altogether this makes for an extremely readable book. Also content-wise there is much of interest. For one, the book establishes that the distribution of pronominal gender forms in the varieties of English is much more complex, and hence much more interesting, than one might think at first sight. As with any other publication, some issues remain that deserve further discussion, two of which will be addressed below.
In my opinion, the kind of variation data used by the author are at the same time the strongest and weakest part of the book. It is astonishing to see the kind of robust patterns recurring in such a diverse collection of data sets which have hardly anything in common in terms of genre, style, register or the way in which the data have been collected. The emergence of such clear generalizations from such a heterogeneous data set seems to strongly support the grammatical relevance and reality of the phenomenon under discussion. One can, however, bargain about the presentation, interpretation and comparison of the different data sets examined. My main objection concerns the fact that the author only presents tables with counts and percentages but does not corroborate his findings by the use of statistics. Instead the reader is often presented with impressionistic descriptions of the data and qualifications such as ''there is no significant (in the non-technical sense) difference'' (p. 111) which are rendered rather meaningless without a proper grounding. Although I am aware that statistical modeling is not everyone's cup of tea, I believe the data analysis and conclusions presented in the book could have benefited from some descriptive statistics and simple tests (chi-square, Fisher's exact). More importantly, given that the different data sets are hardly comparable and numbers within data sets often highly skewed, application of such methods would have allowed the author to draw stronger conclusions than he was able to do now in many instances.
Statistical methods might also have been of help in finding a solution to the second point of discussion which is the definition of the notion of individuation. Although the concept of individuation is very central to the book, its definition seems to change from chapter to chapter. As stated in chapter 1 and detailed in chapter 7 individuation is conceptualized in terms of a hierarchy ranging from proper names to mass nouns, cf. above. The distinction in the hierarchy most relevant to pronominal usage in the varieties of English is that between count and mass nouns. However, in many instances this distinction does not suffice and further parameters, with no place on the hierarchy, are needed, e.g. the notion of mobile in Newfoundland English. In chapter 8 the author himself notes that maintaining his line of argumentation in terms of individuation ''necessitates a concept of individuation that significantly goes beyond undisputed notions (or qualities) like discreteness or boundedness ... Many of the factors discussed there can plausibly be subsumed under the notion of 'individuation''' (p. 163). Instead of subsuming all kinds of different factors under the notion of individuation one could also keep this notion simple and investigate how its interaction with these other factors results in the observed patterns. Although under the latter scenario it may seem that there is no unified explanation for the phenomenon of pronoun usage, one can wonder how unified the explanation is with a broad notion of individuation. Whichever way one chooses, and I would opt for the latter, I think statistical methods may help to elucidate and to better understand pronominal usage in the varieties of English.
The kind of micro-typological approach advocated by the author is still in its infancy. In his book he has demonstrated that the combination of this methodology with a variationist approach can result in new and interesting facts about the grammar of a well-studied language like English. These empirical results make a strong case for the methods applied in the book to be used in future research. I am convinced that taking into account the remarks raised above this will yield many more interesting results.
REFERENCES Elworthy, F.T. (1886)[1965]. _The West Somerset Word-Book and Glossary_. Vaduz: Kraus Reprint LTD.
MacKay, D. and T. Konishi. (1980). Personification and the pronoun problem. _Women’s Studies International Quarterly_ 3, 149-163.
Matthiot, M. (1979). Sex roles as revealed through referential gender in American English. In: M. Matthiot (ed.), _Ethnolinguistics: Boas, Sapir and Whorf revisited_. The Hague: Mouton, 1-47.
Paddock, H. (1991). The actuation problem for gender change in Wessex and Newfoundland. In: P. Trudgill and J. K. Chambers (eds), _Dialects of English: Studies in Grammatical Variation_. London: Longman, 29-46.
Svartengren, T. (1927). The femine gender for inanimate things in Anglo-American. _American Speech_ 3, 83-113.
Wagner, S. (2004). Gender in English Pronouns: Myth and Reality. http://deposit.ddb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv?idn=97211100x.
ABOUT THE REVIEWER Peter de Swart received his PhD in Linguistics from the Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands in November 2007. His dissertation entitled ''Cross-linguistic variation in object marking'' examines the influence of semantic features, in particular animacy, on the marking of direct objects. Until the end of 2008 he continued his research as a postdoctoral researcher in an NWO financed project 'animacy' at the same university. Since January 2009 he is a postdoctoral researcher at the Center for Language and Cognition Groningen of the University of Groningen, the Netherlands. In his NWO VENI-project ''The status of hierarchies in language production and comprehension'' he investigates the role of hierarchies of individuation in grammar and use.
|