EDITORS: Kristiansen, Gitte; Dirven, René TITLE: Cognitive Sociolinguistics SUBTITLE: Language Variation, Cultural Models, Social Systems SERIES: Cognitive Linguistics Research 39 PUBLISHER: Mouton de Gruyter YEAR: 2008
Marissa Fond, Department of Linguistics, Georgetown University
SUMMARY
The 15 articles compiled in ''Cognitive Sociolinguistics: Language Variation, Cultural Models, Social Systems'' grew out of presentations at the 30th International LAUD (Linguistic Agency University of Duisburg) Symposium at the University of Koblenz-Landau in Germany, where participants discussed the need for new research approaches that would bring together the methods and goals of cognitive linguistics and sociolinguistics. Cognitive linguistics is a discipline that has formed the basis of robust lines of research in other established areas of linguistics such as first language acquisition (e.g. Tomasello 2003), second language acquisition (e.g. Tyler 2008), and neuroscience (e.g. Lamb 1999). The editors of this volume aim to add sociolinguistics to this list. Work in cognitive linguistics had often been carried out with a homogenous, idealized speech community as a model, and this practice did not adequately consider the potential contributions that the study of linguistic variation could make to cognitive linguistics, a field that takes a usage-based approach to language study. The volume takes a broad view of the burgeoning field of cognitive sociolinguistics and groups the chapters into four thematic sections: (1) theoretical work on semantics and lexis; (2) usage- and corpus-based work on variation; (3) research on cultural models; and (4) research on political and socioeconomic systems.
The first section addresses why cognitive sociolinguistics is relevant to cognitive linguistic theories of prototypes and stereotypes. In cognitive linguistic tradition, grammar is considered functional and modeled after usage; so it would make sense that social information must be taken into account whenever usage is analyzed. For instance, in one of the canonical semantic theories of cognitive linguistics, prototype theory (e.g. Rosch 1975), the stereotypes and semantic norms that inform this theory are not clearly defined, and the idealized notion of a homogenous speech community is not relevant for refining these definitions because the same stereotypes are not necessarily held by all speakers of a language or dialect. Sociolinguistic research might be useful in determining what stereotypes exist and how they are used in different speech communities. In ''Prototypes, stereotypes, and semantic norms,'' Dirk Geeraerts discusses semantic norms as determined by quantitative analysis of the distribution of the meanings of prototypes and stereotypes in diverse speech communities. He shows how cognitive linguistic theories can be strengthened by sociolinguistic research on what speakers with different linguistic experiences consider to be core semantic concepts. Likewise, sociolinguistic phenomena can be reanalyzed from a cognitive linguistic perspective; this idea is put forth in the chapter by Gitte Kristiansen, ''Style-shifting and shifting styles: A socio-cognitive approach to lectal variation,'' which discusses how variation in phonology, morphology, and semantics can be studied from a cognitive linguistic perspective. For example, the first part of the chapter examines the phenomenon of accent, which is not often discussed in cognitive linguistics, and the social meanings it evokes. The author argues that phonemic contrasts among accents index prototype categories that are related to social identities. The second part of the chapter shows how these phenomena can be used more actively by speakers to position themselves within the network of prototype categories.
In the second section, cognitive linguistics as a usage-based theory of language is discussed in the context of large corpus studies. The editors claim that if cognitive linguistics is usage based, then corpus data are a necessary component of cognitive linguistic research because these natural language data (in contrast to an abstract, ideal speech community) are influenced by the social factors relevant to the given speakers. The chapters in this section include quantitative analyses of corpus data and present meta-analyses of the cognitive linguistic and sociolinguistic methodologies that are applicable to these types of data. Essentially, in this section 'sociolinguistics' is taken to mean quantitative analysis of naturally-occurring speech. In ''Methodological issues in corpus-based Cognitive Linguistics,'' Kris Heylen, José Tummers, and Dirk Geeraerts compare two different empirical methodologies for working with corpus data, examining syntactic variation. In ''Channel and constructional meaning: A collostructional case study,'' Anatol Stefanowitsch and Stefan Th. Gries respond to the criticism of their research methodology presented in the previous chapter, and suggest how sociolinguistic variables like register and channel can be incorporated into quantitative corpus studies. In ''National variation in the use of 'er' 'there': Regional and diachronic constraints on cognitive explanations,'' Stefan Grondelaers, Dirk Speelman and Dirk Geeraerts challenge the common belief that cognitive mechanisms correspond exactly with observed linguistic variation. Specifically, they show that in the case of Dutch 'er', variation in the syntactic position of the word does not indicate a functional difference. Finally, in ''Variation in the choice of adjectives in the two main national varieties of Dutch,'' Dirk Speelman, Stefan Grondelaers, and Dirk Geeraerts compare the choice of adjectives in two varieties of Dutch, analyzing corpora from different regions using a word frequency list to find variation patterns. In sum, the chapters in this section are important for their concentration on the role of corpora in empirical research; however, the methodology presented in the papers in this section should be characterized as a small subset of the methods of sociolinguistic inquiry. The section does not mention the sociolinguistic methods for investigating variation that are not corpus based; there are methods that are quantitative on a smaller scale, and even qualitative (see e.g. Thelander 1982), and perhaps these methods will be included alongside corpus studies in future collections of usage-based research.
In the following two sections, the volume changes course. The first two sections were mostly concerned with how the existence of linguistic variation affects the core tenets of cognitive linguistics, and how the quantitative, corpus-based methods used in sociolinguistics could be useful for research in cognitive linguistics; but the chapters in section 3 change the focus around, outlining how cognitive linguistics can be applied not to sociolinguistic research methods or theories but to social issues of language use. Likewise, section 4 critically applies cognitive linguistic ideas to social and political systems. These sections introduce the reader to a new subfield of sociolinguistics, one that seems more in keeping with the term 'cognitive sociolinguistics' rather than 'quantitative sociolinguistic methods applied to cognitive linguistics'.
The third section discusses cognitive cultural models, which have not been widely researched within the field of cognitive linguistics, and focuses specifically on cultural models as they relate to language policy. In ''Rationalist or Romantic models in globalization,'' Frank Polzenhagen and René Dirven discuss how globalization affects the content of languages, specifically global English. They compare two Western cultural models and apply them to language: the rationalist model is ''language as a tool'' and the romantic model is ''language as an identity marker''. These models are argued to underlie the current discussions of linguistic globalization. In ''A nation is a territory with one culture and one language: The role of metaphorical folk models in language policy debates,'' Raphael Berthele focuses on language ideologies in Switzerland and the United States and how foreign language is discussed in light of the underlying metaphors for language used. For example, he considers the English-Only movement in the United States and the metaphors that proponents and opponents of this idea employ in order to argue their points and influence policy. In ''Cultural models of Home in Aboriginal children's English,'' Farzad Sharifian discusses how models differ across ethnic groups who speak the same language. As an example, the word 'home' has two different meanings for Aboriginal and Anglo-Australian children speaking English; in brief, the Aboriginal children considered 'home' to include members of their family, while the Anglo-Australian children considered only the physical building to mean 'home'. Sharifian shows that though all of the children studied spoke the same dialect of English, the cultural models underlying their language were influenced by their ethnicities, and these models are constantly changing. In ''A Cognitive Linguistic approach to the cultures of World Englishes: The emergence of a new model,'' Hans-Georg Wolf highlights the role of cultural models in the development of varieties of World Englishes, and shows how culture is expressed in each variety. As was discussed in the previous chapter, Wolf notes that while some elements of English are the same across different varieties, many of the concepts underlying these formal structures differ by culture.
The fourth and final section includes examinations of social and political issues in institutional contexts, which has long been an active domain of cognitive linguistics within academia and outside of it as well (e.g. George Lakoff's now-closed Rockridge Institute). In ''Corporate brands as socio-cognitive representations,'' Veronika Koller suggests how business media discourse constructs corporate identities through mission statements that revolve around the concepts of partnership and emotion, with the goal of improving the image of the corporation. In ''Metaphorically speaking: Gender and classroom discourse,'' Susan Fiksdal shows how the metaphorical expressions differ when used by college-aged men and women to characterize a seminar. Men typically expressed thoughts that employed the underlying metaphor 'seminar is a game' while the women's thoughts employed 'seminar is a community', which leads the author to conclude that while both genders expressed a desire to collaborate, their conceptualizations of what collaboration meant varied. In ''The business model of the university: Sources and consequences of its construal,'' Nancy Urban shows how the popularized idea of a university as a business, as seen in a selection of university-related texts, is understood through a Darwinian 'natural selection' metaphor. In that the business world and the free market are games of survival, the university system is becoming more closely related to this underlying concept. In ''Competition, cooperation, and interconnection: 'Metaphor families' and social systems,'' Pamela S. Morgan shows how social institutions are characterized by the three metaphors in the title of the paper. Specifically, she describes how different facets of the business world and the political world are understood through these metaphor families. And in the final chapter, ''How cognitive linguists can help to solve political problems,'' Karol Janicki discusses her 'non-essentialist' view of definitions of words, which means that one true definition (to the exclusion of all others) is not a realistic possibility. In her discussion of the Monica Lewinsky scandal, the American election of 2000 vote count, and the stem cell research question, she argues that by taking a non-essentialist view of definitions, politicians can work the inherent 'fuzziness' of definitions to their advantage.
EVALUATION
This volume touches on many subfields of cognitive linguistics and cognitive sociolinguistics, and successfully makes the case that cognitive sociolinguistics is a paradigm in which future researchers can work productively. As there has been some opposition of late to traditional cognitive linguistic approaches that do not consider cognitive concepts to be influenced by social factors, there has been increased interest in variationist perspectives. As mentioned above, many cognitive linguists have come to eschew the idea of basing future research on an abstract, homogenous speech community, in favor of attending to the challenges of working with naturally-occurring speech. The main point of the volume that each section underscores in a distinct way is that in order for cognitive linguistics to productively examine language usage, and work within a heterogeneous speech community rather than an idealized abstraction, the field must incorporate methods and theories from sociolinguistics.
As the editors make clear, this volume is one of the earliest published collections of research on cognitive sociolinguistics. As such, it makes sense that the chapters would cover many different topics and approach the incorporation of sociolinguistics in diverse ways. The decision to divide the volume into four sections, organized by theme, is a very good one; it creates order and a clearer purpose in what might have been a fragmented collection. The editors' introduction outlines the goals of each section and sets up the background for the chapters contained within, which is crucial to a volume such as this one. However, the excellent introduction could be improved by more attention paid to the field of sociolinguistics. The editors provide clear, detailed background on cognitive linguistics, including the development of the discipline and its future; sociolinguistics should receive a similar treatment, as this would help the reader to better understand the goals of the volume. For example, the fact that the four sections involve very different approaches to sociolinguistics and social institutions, and consider different aspects of the field as being useful in cognitive sociolinguistics, indicates that a more thorough introduction to cognitive linguists' take on sociolinguistics would be helpful; this would better background the chapters and give the disciplines of cognitive linguistics and sociolinguistics more comparable weight.
Lastly, in this volume, there is less consideration of how theories from cognitive linguistics could be incorporated into sociolinguistics to create new avenues of research in that field; other than in the Kristiansen paper, the volume mainly discusses sociolinguistics and its methods in the service of cognitive linguistics. This is very reasonable, given the backgrounds of the authors and editors, and that the volume belongs to the series ''Cognitive Linguistics Research''. But in the introduction the editors write: ''Research that endeavors to unravel, examine, and compare social and cognitive dimensions can in a most natural way be subsumed under the cover term cognitive sociolinguistics'' (p. 4), and this egalitarian statement about the field of cognitive sociolinguistics is not quite fully manifested. Given the strong support of cognitive sociolinguistics by the LAUD Symposia, it is likely that the field of cognitive sociolinguistics will grow; it has already done so since the publication of this volume. Perhaps in the next volume, more papers from a primarily sociolinguistic perspective can be included with those from a cognitive background, and the interdisciplinary field of cognitive sociolinguistics will become increasingly robust and relevant.
REFERENCES
Lamb, S. (1999). Pathways of the Brain: The Neurocognitive Basis of Language. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192-233.
Thelander, M. (1982). A qualitative approach to the quantitative data of speech variation. In Suzanne Romaine (Ed.), Sociolinguistic Variation in Speech Communities (pp. 65-83). London: Edward Arnold.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tyler, A. (2008). Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Instruction. In N. Ellis & P. Robinson (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition (pp. 456-488). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
ABOUT THE REVIEWER
|