Date: Tue, 8 Jun 2004 12:50:28 +0200 From: Susana Perales Haya Subject: Language Competence Across Populations: Toward a Definition of SLI
EDITORS: Levy, Yonata; Schaeffer, Jeannette TITLE: Language Competence Across Populations SUBTITLE: Toward a Definition of Specific Language Impairment PUBLISHER: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates YEAR: 2003
Susana Perales, University of the Basque Country (Spain)
INTRODUCTION
The editors of the book have gathered in this volume 17 articles (chapters) that were presented at a workshop on Specific Language Impairment (SLI) held in Jerusalem in 2000. The book is divided into Part A, which comprises 15 chapters and whose title is ''Language competence across populations'' and Part B, ''Toward a definition of SLI?''. The first part is subsequently subdivided into three sections: The first one is ''The characterization of Specific Language Impairment'' (chapters 1-7), the second one is ''Methodological Concerns'' (chapters 8-11) and finally, the third section is entitled ''Language competence in children with neurodevelopmental disorders'' (chapters 12-15).
As the editors themselves declare in the preface, the book's focus is on ''the question of how much variability linguistic competence can take in children as they develop'' (p. x). Thus, although the focal theme is SLI, other language disorders (e.g. language in autistic children or children with Williams Syndrome) are also portrayed and discussed in the book.
SUMMARY
The first chapter ''Lenneberg's dream: Learning, normal language development, and specific language impairment'' (Ken Wexler) explores the interrelation between the study of SLI and normal language acquisition. Wexler first introduces the Optional Infinitive (OI) stage in normally developing children, which is characterized by the production of an infinitive form in contexts where the adult grammar requieres an inflected verb. Wexler explains the OI stage as the result of a limitation in the child's computational system which prevents him from checking more than once against the D-feature of noun phrases. The ''Unique Checking Constraint'' (UCC), as Wexler calls it, ''is a developmental constraint on the computational system of language'' (p. 33) and not only gives a linguistic account of the OI stage, but also predicts that children acquiring null subject languages (e.g. Spanish, Italian) do not go through this stage, as has been reported in the literature (see Wexler 1998 for an overview). As for how normally developing children recover from the OI stage, it is proposed that ''it matures away, under genetic guidance'' (p. 40), and Wexler reviews several arguments that support this proposal.
As far as SLI is concerned, Wexler explains how the proposal to account for OIs in normal children can also be used for SLI children. Thus, he claims that SLI children go through an Extended Optional Infinitive (EOI) stage, implying that the maturation responsible for the withering away of the UCC in normal children does not operate or is delayed in SLI children. In this way, both normal and impaired language development can be accounted for with the same theoretical apparatus, something highly desirable in linguistic theory. Moreover, as Wexler makes explicit all throughout the article, linguistic-based accounts are preferable to other proposals that place the child with no specific knowledge of linguistic categories but only with a set of general cognitive learning mechanisms (e.g. Tomasello 2000, cited in the article), with the result that the phenomena related to the OI stage are left unexplained.
In chapter 2 ''A unified model of specific and general language delay: Grammatical tense as a clinical marker of unexpected variation'', Mabel Rice deals with tense marking as a diagnosis of SLI and places emphasis on distinguishing between a selective delay in language acquisition, which is indicative of a deficit in an element of grammar, and a general delay, which does not necessarily imply that a particular aspect of grammar is affected.
Rice reports on various longitudinal studies of English- speaking SLI children. First, she deals with grammatical tense marking (e.g. third person ''-s'') and her results show that for the SLI group, the variation expected at age 5 falls below that of normally developing children. In order to examine whether the problem with grammatical tense marking is a reflection of a more general deficit affecting low salient morphemes, Rice compared performance on the plural marker ''-s'' and found that all groups of children used this marker productively, which denotes that grammar, rather than phonology, is involved in SLI. However, that wasn't the case in lexical acquisition and mean length of utterance (MLU) measures, where SLI children seemed to be lagging behind the normal controls, and no particular aspect of grammar was affected.
When comparing SLI children versus children with Williams Syndrome, Rice found that they performed similarly in MLU, plurals and prepositions, but the SLI group performed significantly worse on tense marking. This suggests that grammatical tense marking is a reliable clinical marker. To conclude, Rice points out that, in order to decide whether low performance in grammatical tense marking is indicative of a general language delay or a selective language delay, other measures (e.g. MLU, lexical development, etc.) must be taken into consideration.
In the third chapter (''Two of a kind? The importance of commonalities and variation across languages and learners'') Martha Crago and Johanne Paradis examine how SLI manifests itself with regard to verbal morphology in three languages: English, French and Inuktitut, and in three different populations: normally developing children, SLI children and second language learners. Crago and Paradis raise several issues regarding the Extended Optional Infinitive stage proposed by Wexler (chapter 1). The matters they discuss concern the ultimate attainment of SLI affected children, the fact that SLI does not manifest in the same way across languages, and that SLI children do not always behave like normal language-matched controls. They highlight the importance of figuring out if SLI children reveal simply a delay with respect to normal language acquisition or whether it would be better to speak of a deviant language acquisition process. Also, they cast doubt on the appropriateness of the term ''Optional infinitive'' because SLI errors across languages normally involve the use of default rather than infinitive forms.
Chapter 4 ''Do heterogeneous deficits require heterogeneous theories? SLI subgroups and the RDDR hypothesis'' (Heather K. J. van der Lely) deals with the controversy surrounding the domain-specific versus domain-general accounts of SLI. Some scholars regard evidence of SLI children affected in phonological or pragmatic measures as indicative that SLI is not a domain-specific language deficit (e.g. Karmiloff- Smith 1998, cited in the article). However, after reviewing research studies exploring different language abilities within homogeneous subgroups of SLI children, Van der Lely argues that ''the simplest explanation for the association between disorders is the propensity for comorbidity of disorders in development'' (p. 123).
Van der Lely goes on to report the results of her study of a homogeneous SLI sub-group: the Grammatical SLI subgroup, which is characterized by deficits in tense and agreement marking along with other problems with syntactic operations like, for instance, theta-role assignment or embedded clauses. The author claims that Grammatical SLI is caused by a deficit involving the obligatoriness of movement operations. This proposal allows her to account for a wide range of syntactic phenomena that characterize the Grammatical SLI subgroup performance.
In chapter 5, Jeannette Schaeffer addresses the issue of modularity within the human language faculty. More specifically, she concentrates on the study of pragmatics as an independent module from the lexicon and the computational system. For this purpose, she investigates the performance of Dutch SLI and normally-developing children on object scrambling, a syntactic phenomenon which is triggered by pragmatics. Schaeffer entertains the hypothesis that SLI children are impaired in their grammar, but not in their pragmatics and therefore, the application of object scrambling will not be affected in these children. Her results support this hypothesis as Dutch SLI children do correctly scramble referential objects 96% of the time. Thus, she concludes, the pragmatic module is not affected in SLI, which is evidence that pragmatics and the computational system are independent modules within the human language faculty.
In Chapter 6 (''Specific language impairment and linguistic explanation'') Jan de Jong considers the issue of the different predictions and explanations that stem from distinct theoretical proposals. De Jong reports the results of an experimental study with Dutch SLI children which shows that the developmental order of acquisition of finiteness in English is not comparable to the Dutch order. Then, he goes on to propose an alternative explanation of the OI stage (Wexler, chapter 1), namely the ''Modal drop hypothesis'' (Ingram & Thompson 1996), which claims that children OI sentences should be interpreted as containing a null modal. The chapter ends with the observation that the same surface phenomenon can have diverse theoretical interpretations, each of which differing in the degree of knowledge the SLI child is credited with.
Chapter 7 is the last chapter of the first section and its title is ''The role of language typology in linguistic development: Implications for the study of language disorders'' (Dorit Ravid, Ronit Levie and Galit Avivi Ben- Zvi). These authors explore the knowledge of Hebrew derivational morphology by SLI children and normally developing controls. An experimental study was conducted on comprehension and production of nouns and adjectives, which showed that SLI children performed significantly lower than normal controls in most cases. Thus, the authors assert that ''derivational morphology was found to be diagnostic of the impaired population'' (p. 189). They also underscore the importance of looking at typologically diverse languages, and including measures like comprehension in order to gain a greater understanding of SLI.
In chapter 8 (''Specific language impairment: characterizing the deficits''), Laurence B. Leonard discusses several methodological questions with regard to the study of SLI focusing on three broad areas: definition of SLI, research design and hypothesis testing, and data interpretation. These issues are central to a well known fact in SLI research: children with SLI are far from being a homogeneous group. This poses a problem for replicability, as two studies focusing on the same linguistic aspect may obtain different results due to the intrinsic characteristics of the sample they look at. Thus, Leonard proposes the inclusion of descriptive data so as to facilitate subject selection, replication and identification of subgroups. The author also discusses the convenience of several research methods, such as group versus individual case studies, group-matching criteria, the importance of designing studies that take into account how SLI children are affected by different types of treatment, as well as the need for longitudinal studies. Finally, the chapter ends with the discussion of how different hypotheses may lead to contrasting conclusions, stressing that ''each discipline's basic assumptions should be spelled out, measures should be compatible with these assumptions, and null hypotheses should be framed in a uniform manner'' (p. 228)
Chapter 9 (''Methodological issues in cross-group comparisons of language and cognitive development'' by Carolyn B. Mervis and Byron F. Robinson) discusses methodological questions regarding group-matching procedures, chronological age confounds and other problems stemming from cross-group comparisons. They also discuss an alternative method: profiling, which implies, firstly, to determine whether a given linguistic profile is characteristic of a particular child and, secondly, to determine how characteristic/uncharacteristic that profile is of the children who are/are not in the SLI group.
The main theme discussed in chapter 10 (''MLU-matching and the production of morphosyntax in Dutch children with specific language impairment'' by Gerard W. Bol) is the use of MLU measures in SLI studies. The author illustrates some of the problems arising from the use of MLU as a marker of productive morphosyntax as well as its use as a means to match different groups of subjects, and he also puts forward suggestions that may help to overcome these difficulties. In the second part of the article, the author conducts a study on Dutch children which attempts to figure out how Dutch SLI children compensate for loss of utterance length. He identified seven variables like, for instance, the use of conjunctions, prepositions and adverbials, verb forms used for indicating past tense, etc. However, none of the phenomena he looked at seemed to be used by Dutch SLI children to compensate for loss of sentence length. He concludes that MLU is not a valid measuring strategy for Dutch, and calls for alternatives to MLU.
In chapter 11 (''Different methodologies yield incongruous results: A study of the spontaneous use of verb forms in Hebrew'' by Esther Dromi, Laurence B. Leonard & Anat Blass) spontaneous and elicited production data from Hebrew SLI children and normally developing controls are compared focusing on verb morphology. The first part of the article is devoted to the review of published results obtained in the investigation of verbal morphology using a series of tasks specially designed to elicit specific verb forms. The use of tasks allowed the researchers to manipulate variables and concentrate on particular aspects regarding performance on verbal morphology.
In the second part of the article, the authors compare the results obtained in the elicitation studies with spontaneous production data. One of the strengths of the study is that the SLI children and the controls were the same in both the elicited and the spontaneous production studies. The authors resolve that both tasks are necessary tools for the study of SLI and thus should be combined to get an accurate picture of SLI children's performance.
In Chapter 12 (''Language impairment in children with complex neurodevelopmental disorders'') Helen Tager-Flusberg examines the linguistic differences and similarities between autistic and SLI children. The author describes an experimental longitudinal study in which two samples from the groups mentioned above were compared on a series of phonological, lexical and grammatical tests. The results point to the fact that within the group of autistic children there was a subgroup which performed similarly to the SLI group, which leads the author to highlight that autism and SLI are ''overlapping populations'' (p. 311). The results of this study bear directly on current research which is showing that there may be a shared gene in autism and SLI.
Harald Clahsen and Christine Temple take up the issue of the modularity of the mind in chapter 13 (''Words and rules in children with Williams syndrome''). Their study measures performance of children with Williams syndrome on comparative adjectives, receptive / productive vocabulary and reading skills. They aim to show that modular accounts are preferable to other non-modular connectionist models, as they are able to explain a wider range of effects.
In chapter 14 (''Basic language skills in children with neurodevelopmental disorders and the notion of brain plasticity'') Yonata Levy studies the performance of several groups of children with diverse neurodevelopmental disorders on a series of grammatical tasks, as well as their response to requests for clarification. A first study was conducted to examine the linguistic performance of 8 children with neurodevelopmental disorders on 13 variables like syntactic agreement, gender marking, etc. The language problems found in these children reflected a delayed although not different linguistic profile with respect to normally developing children.
The second study assessed children's response to clarification requests. Roughly, it was found that the ability of children with neurodevelopmental disorders to detect errors in their own speech as response to clarification requests matches the pattern seen in normally developing children. Results show that, up to an MLU of 3, there is not much variation in the course of language development irrespective of the population, and Levy suggests this may be due to brain plasticity assuring a basic level of language performance.
Signed and spoken languages are compared in chapter 15 (''On the complementarity of signed and spoken languages'') by Wendy Sandler. She considers the idea that sign and spoken language are two parts of a unique human language faculty. After reviewing some of the similarities and differences between sign and spoken language, the author shows evidence from gesture that supports her proposal. Gesture, as the author claims, is a natural part of communication, both for deaf signers (who gesture with the mouth) and for oral speakers (who gesture with their hands), and its study should lead to a deeper understanding of the human language faculty.
The second part of the book (''Toward a definition of SLI?'') comprises the last two chapters. Chapter 16 (''Understanding SLI: A neuropsychological perspective'' by Dorit Ben Shalom) considers how the study of SLI can benefit from studies of other cognitive disorders and vice versa. In the first place, the author compares SLI with prosopagnosia (impaired face recognition) and claims that the fact that there are specific grammatical deficits in SLI does not necessarily imply that SLI should be defined in terms of exclusionary criteria. Secondly, Shalom compares three phenomena related to SLI and agrammatism: tense marking, reversible passives and nonword repetition. She suggests that there may be two types of grammatical processing, one phonological and one syntactic, and states that the study of both should guide us to provide an accurate answer to the general problem of grammatical processing in the brain.
In the last chapter (''Defining SLI: A linguistic perspective'') Jill G. de Villiers takes up several issues that have been discussed in the articles gathered in the book: (i) the question of whether SLI children are a different group from other cognitively impaired children, (ii) SLI as a reflection of a specific problem in the language module, (iii) the heterogeneity of SLI and, lastly, (iv) whether SLI is better described as a deficit or a delay. De Villiers does an excellent job at summarizing and commenting the key points raised by the articles in the book, as well as suggesting further lines of investigation.
EVALUATION
As a whole, the book provides an excellent overview of current issues concerning research on SLI. For the most part, the authors have been carrying out research on SLI for quite a number of years, which is an extraordinary opportunity to get first-hand knowledge of the topics that have been debated and the answers that have been advanced so far with regard to SLI. Another outstanding issue regards the fact that all contributors have read each other's articles and, thus, they refer the reader to particular chapters where the same issues are discussed. This contributes to maintain a coherent line and helps the reader to situate arguments and positions within the SLI research framework.
The book is well suited for scholars doing research on SLI because they will get an overall picture of the field as it stands nowadays, as well as because there are specific proposals that suggest new lines of investigation. This is particularly true of the section on methodological problems, as the chapters herein included spot specific methodological flaws that should be overcome in future studies. Graduate students with a fair command of linguistic theory will also find the book interesting, as well as those acquainted with the literature on normal language acquisition.
To conclude, I consider this volume is an outstanding contribution not only for the answers it provides, but also for the questions it leaves open for further research.
REFERENCES
Ingram, D. & Thompson, W. (1996) ''Early syntactic acquisition in German: Evidence for the modal hypothesis'' Language 72: 97-120.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998) ''Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders''. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2: 389-398.
Tomasello, M. (2000) ''Do young children have adult syntactic competence?'' Cognition 74: 209-253.
Wexler, K. (1998) ''Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage'' Lingua 106: 23-79.
|