Michael Hammond. The Phonology of English: A Prosodic Optimality-Theoretic Approach. Oxford:Oxford University Press. 1999. 368pp.
Reviewed by David Deterding
Synopsis
This book presents a detailed analysis of the syllable structure and stress placement of English words, using Optimality Theory (OT) as the theoretical framework for the analysis. Because of the complexity of these issues, the consideration of stress placement concentrates almost entirely on monomorphemic words, so only seven pages (pp.322-329) are devoted to the influences of suffixes on stress placement, and issues of what happens to words in context are not covered.
Chapter 1 introduces the way that OT provides a mechanism for considering the interaction of various phonological constraints, using aspiration and vowel nasalisation as processes to illustrate the use of OT tableaux. Chapters 2 to 4 then consider the structure of the English syllable, with a detailed analysis of phonotactic constraints and also the use of moras to express constraints on syllable structure. Chapters 5 to 8 discuss the theory of the foot and the complex interaction of various constraints in determining the placement of stress in monomorphemic English words. Finally, Chapter 9 presents a brief overview, and also a consideration of further areas that deserve to be researched.
Critical Evaluation
(Note: Hammond uses [s-hacek, z-hacek] for the coronal-dorsal fricatives of English. For typographical reasons, here I will instead use the pseudo-IPA symbols [S, Z] for these sounds.)
Hammond's book is written very clearly, with issues concerning the OT representation of syllable structure and stress placement in monomorphemic English words examined meticulously and exhaustively. The book thus not only presents a comprehensive analysis of these complex issues, but it also provides a welcome overview of OT, and thereby demonstrates the power of this theoretical framework. This book is certain to become an important work on English phonology, and I think everyone should read it.
In the following paragraphs, I will raise some issues that I feel merit further discussion. In many ways, my discussion of these issues may actually be regarded as a tribute to the clarity of the book: it is always highly informative and thoroughly thought-provoking, and the fact that it encourages one to delve further into many of the issues and maybe question some of the conclusions is a result of the overall lucidity of the presentation.
Although most issues are clearly and carefully presented, there are a few which are introduced a little abruptly. For example, it is a bit alarming to be told (p.172) that the Nonfinality Constraint "prevents the final syllable from being footed", as any speaker of English will immediately be able to think of plenty of examples where the final syllable of a word is not just footed, but stressed as well. In this and other similar cases, such abrupt presentation of an idea that initially seems counter-intuitive is mitigated by the subsequent meticulous use of a large number of OT tableaux to show how the various constraints interact to allow the correct derivation of the metrical stress structure of words. Indeed, after this detailed presentation, not only does the nature and ranking of the constraints become apparent, but the strength of the OT framework emerges extraordinarily clearly.
Sometimes, however, the subsequent explanation of an issue really is delayed too long. We are told (p.250) that there are virtually no clear cases of disyllabic monomorphemic words ending in a velar nasal. On reading this, words like 'pudding' and 'herring' immediately come to mind, and it is only 23 pages later (p.273) that we are told that such words are being treated as if they have an -ing suffix. Not only is this delay unfortunate, I also don't find the treatment totally convincing: while it would seem perfectly reasonable to argue that the frequency of the -ing suffix has had a substantial effect on the syllable structure of English words, including monomorphemic words, I have residual doubts about the suggestion that 'pudding' and 'herring' somehow consist of two morphemes, or indeed that 'honest' and 'earnest' might also be treated as polymorphemic because they have an -est suffix (p.252).
One final comment about the abrupt introduction of new concepts: this book assumes quite an extensive background knowledge of phonetics and some phonology. While it might be reasonable to assume that a reader is already familiar with aspiration and anticipatory vowel nasality, the introduction of the 'sonority hierarchy' with no explanation (p.85: "I propose that margins are subject to the sonority hierarchy") might prove something of a barrier to those not familiar with such concepts -- surely a brief definition of sonority would be useful.
This book covers some things in impressive detail, while other matters are perhaps skipped over rather lightly. For example, it is simply assumed that feet in English are trochees (one stressed syllable followed by one unstressed syllable), and no consideration is given to the possibility of dactyls (one stressed syllable followed by two unstressed syllables) even though this term is introduced on p.150. As a result, when all the potential ways of parsing a 3-syllable word such as 'Christopher' are listed, a dactylic analysis is not considered, and the final analysis that emerges consists of a trochee followed by an unparsed final syllable. While there is (no doubt) plenty of evidence to support this analysis, as it is presented in this book it is not totally convincing. Indeed, while the OT tableaux illustrate admirably how the constraints can be ranked to achieve the correct parsing, the nagging suspicion remains that some of the constraints are ad hoc: not only is there the failure to consider analysis in terms of a dactyl, and also the abruptness of the presentation of the Nonfinality Constraint discussed above, but I additionally feel the suggestion that many verbs and nouns have a catalectic (silent) suffix (p.278) needs further justification to supplement the demonstration that the existence of such a suffix can provide the right answers for the stress of English words.
Another issue that I feel could benefit from further attention concerns the status of the coronal-dorsal fricative [Z]. As evidence for the existence of the syllable, it is claimed that medial phonotactic constraints emerge from a combination of syllable-final and syllable-initial constraints, but the status of [Z] in English would seem to represent something of a question-mark regarding this claim. Clearly there is no problem with this sound in medial position ('measure', 'leisure'), but in initial position it only occurs in a few borrowed words ('genre', 'gigolo') and in final position in a few more borrowed words ('mirage', 'beige'). Hammond deals with this by simply assuming that it belongs in syllable-final position, and he lists 'liege', 'beige', 'rouge', 'loge', and 'garage' as words ending in [Z] (p.112). Well, 'beige', 'rouge', and 'loge' all seem to me quite clearly to be borrowed, while 'liege' and 'garage' both have alternatives with [dZ], which strongly suggests the instability of [Z] in final position (while there seems to be no such pressure on the medial [Z] in 'measure' or 'leisure'). I do not wish to suggest that Hammond is necessarily wrong in his analysis, as there are certainly good reasons to regard [Z] as a syllable-final consonant. However, I would have preferred that this problem with the status of [Z] had been mentioned, even if it were simply to acknowledge its existence.
My final comment concerns a suggested omission. In establishing the phonotactic relationships of the English syllable, a vast and impressive array of examples are considered, including such rare words as 'knish' and 'tmesis'. The danger is that this lays the analysis open to the 'what about' objection. My suggestion is: what about 'Sri Lanka'? This is relevant, because [S] and [s] are claimed to be in complementary distribution in initial clusters, with [S] occurring only before [r], while [s] occurs before all other consonants (p.55). While exceptions of Yiddish origin do get mentioned, 'Sri Lanka' does not, and it would certainly appear to be a possible exception (depending on how the first sound is actually pronounced). Now you might object that this is a name, so it doesn't count. But 'Minsk' gets two mentions (pp.65 & 95), and some of the names considered can be really quite obscure: 'Bhutatathata', 'Haleakala', 'Kichisaburo', 'Anaxagorean', 'Antananarivo' .... all on p.300. So why not 'Sri Lanka'?
Summary
I have discussed a number of issues, particularly concerning the brevity with which some issues are considered, and this might seem to suggest that the book has severe flaws. This is not true at all: in fact it presents a complex phonological framework (OT) delightfully clearly, and I thoroughly enjoyed reading it, even if I might have preferred one or two issues to be covered in more detail.
One must accept, however, that not all issues can be covered in depth, and the impressive clarity with which Hammond has presented the phonology of English words within the OT framework is highly commendable.
I highly recommend this book as exceptionally valuable for all students of phonology.
[About the reviewer: David Deterding teaches phonetics, syntax, and translation at the National Institute of Education, NTU, Singapore. His webpage is: http://www.soa.ntu.edu.sg:8080/ell/DavidD/Personal/david.htm]
|