Raimy, Eric (2000) The Phonology and Morphology of Reduplication. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 200pp.
Reviewed by Jason Haugen, University of Arizona
OVERVIEW
Reduplication has been the central topic of much fruitful research within recent work in Prosodic Morphology and Optimality Theory (OT) (McCarthy and Prince 1986, 1995, among many others). In this book Raimy presents a novel approach to reduplication which abandons both the notions of prosody and optimal parallel processing in favor of a unique rule-based derivational account. The primary innovation of Raimy's model is the introduction of an articulated linearization process and the utilization of a looping mechanism prior to linearization to derive the effects of reduplication. Along the way, Raimy tackles most of the classic data sets used by McCarthy and Prince to argue for the Correspondence Theory model, and provides new analyses based on his own modular and derivational model.
SYNOPSIS
This book is an extension of the author's 1999 PhD dissertation, completed at the University of Delaware under William Idsardi. The model developed herein is modular in that it assumes a specific level of Morphological Structure (MS), which is post-syntactic and pre-phonological. This is an assumption that he shares with proponents of Distributed Morphology (DM) (e.g. Halle and Marantz 1993, Harley and Noyer 1999, etc.), the framework in which Raimy is working. In this model, reduplication is treated as a phonological rewrite rule inserted in the morphology, triggered by a 0- morpheme which provides secondary exponence for some morphosyntactic feature. This contrasts with Optimality Theory, where reduplication is generally assumed to be an independent morpheme (RED), which can be manipulated by phonological constraints. The second aspect of the Raimy model is that it is derivational, in that crucial rule orderings derive over- and under-application effects, etc., as we will see below.
At 8 pages, Chapter 1 is very brief and lays out the bare outline of what is to come. The notion of precedence in phonology is made explicit here, and Raimy introduces the notation that he will use in the rest of the book: '#' to mark the beginning of a string, '%' to mark the end, and arrows to mark the precedence relations among the segments of a string. In this model, reduplication is derived by introducing a "loop" into the structure, represented in this work by an arrow from one segment to another. For instance, in the case of full reduplication of the string #->b->u->k-> u->%, an arrow would be drawn from the final [u] back to the [b], and after linearization the string would emerge as: b-u-k-u-b-u-k-u. A syllabic reduplicant would be derived by drawing the arrow from the first [u] back to the [b] (b-u-b- u-k-u), a suffixal syllabic reduplicant by drawing the arrow from the second [u] to the [k] (b-u-k-u-k-u), etc. (This notation had to prove nightmarish in typesetting, and I will not even attempt to replicate it here in text-only format).
Two things are particularly important to note. First, reduplication occurs with the addition of the precedence link in morphology. Linear ordering is a phonological rule which can be ordered with respect to other rules. Second, an "economy condition" rules out infinite loops: i.e. once a loop is followed, it is satisfied.
Chapter 2, "The Phonology of Reduplication", introduces the first case studies which put Raimy's model to work. He starts off by covering over- and under-application. In the over-application case he presents the familiar case of Malay nasal spreading, where the first vowel of the phonological word is nasalized, even though it is not preceded by a nasal segment to trigger such nasalization. In OT, this is accounted for by (and is given as motivation for) constraints necessitating correspondence between base and reduplicant: IDENT-BR[+nasal]. In Raimy's model, however, there is no such notion of correspondence. Instead, the precedence link from the final segment of the word back to the first segment in fact puts the first segment in the relevant environment: it is preceded by a nasal segment, triggering the application of the rule. That is, once the link is added, the first segment actually does follow a nasal segment: it is literally in two places at once.
In under-application, the same thing holds, except that certain conditions are not met, such as the "Uniformity Parameter", which requires that each element of a segment meet the structural requirements of the rule for that rule to apply. For example, in the case of Akan palatalization, non-low front vowels trigger palatalization of dorsal segments, except in reduplication: yielding e.g. [ki-ka?] rather than [tci-tca?] (where c = palatal affricate). Under Raimy's account, this reduplication adds a precedence link with a pre-specified [I] segment, and the initial [k] loops to this [I] and then back to itself, leading to two different precedence relations: one where [k] precedes [I], the other where [k] precedes [a]. The latter case does not trigger the palatalization rule, and since the Uniformity Parameter is set to "on" this rule does not apply in this language. If this parameter had been set to 'off', then we would expect to see forms such as [tci-ka?].
Once again, where OT derives these effects through the notion of base-reduplicant correspondence, Raimy suggests instead that what is occurring is precedence relationships interacting with rules. If linearization occurs before the application of any given rule (such as nasal spread or palatalization), then we see normal application of the rule: the segments involved are no longer identical, but are instead separated through the linearization process. As Raimy notes, his model leads to a nice typology of rule and reduplicant interaction (Raimy's figure (43)). In Chapter 3, "Precedence in Morphology", Raimy further develops his idea that reduplication is a readjustment rule triggered by a 0-morph inserted at morphological structure. In so doing, Raimy points out what he sees as problems for treating a reduplicant (un-phonologically-specified-RED) as an independent morpheme. His primary motivation for the 0- morpheme claim is internal to the strictures of DM: readjustment rules can only be triggered by morphemes providing secondary exponence of a morphosyntactic feature. Raimy asserts that "reduplication in itself is the secondary exponent of a morphosyntactic feature in many cases" (p.61), and even alludes elsewhere that it might be in all cases (p. 66), but the evidence that he gives to support this point is weak. For example, Raimy has a problem with the high frequency of polysemy for reduplicants. He cites the language Nakanai as providing a "particularly clear" (p.63) case of reduplicative polysemy, and quotes Spaelti (1997)'s observation that in Nakanai "the shape of the reduplicated form is independent of the usage. All usages occur with any of the patterns" (p.63). Unfortunately, he only gives four examples:
(1) raga-raga 'jumping'/Cont. Habituative Verbs muluga-luga 'to be first'/Concrete Nouns bolo-bolo 'many pigs'/Collective Plural Nouns ilima-lima 'five'/Distributive Numerals (p.63)
There are two things to note. First, there is only one pattern of reduplication: a foot, so this example set does not show independence of shape and usage, although it does show multiple uses for one shape (i.e. polysemy). Other reduplicative shapes might exist in Nakanai, but Raimy does not provide us with the data.
One place where Raimy might show more of a problem with making a claim that RED is a specific morpheme realizing a specific morphosyntactic feature is in cases where different reduplicant shapes realize the same morphosyntactic feature. A case in point is the Uto-Aztecan language Yaqui (also referred to as Hiaki and Yoeme), where each of the reduplicated verbs forms in (2) signal habitual action:
(2) a. hina 'hoe, chop weeds' -> hin.na b. chepta 'jump over' -> chep.chep.ta c. kinakte 'squint, grimace' -> ki.na.ki.nak.te
The habitual reduplicant (RED) in each of these cases is realized as a bare mora (in a), a syllable (b, where the coda consonant is non-moraic), or a foot (c), none of which is governed by purely phonological conditions (Haugen 2000), which emphasizes Raimy's point elsewhere that a given pattern of reduplication is often specific to a particular lexical item. However, since the morphological gemination and full foot reduplication are limited to a small set of verbs in each case, the syllabic reduplicant seems to be the default, and we can capture this by marking these two cases as exceptional to the more usual pattern, rather than marking every verb for one of the three, as Raimy would have us do.
Second, polysemy is attested elsewhere in morphology, so I do not see why it should be especially problematic for reduplication. For example, the [-z] suffix is the primary exponence of the feature [+plural] in English, and it is also polysemous: reflecting also the genitive and third singular verb agreement. I wouldn't want to make any universal claims about anything based on this pattern, or any other similar case, as Raimy attempts to do with polysemy in reduplication.
Perhaps the most interesting section in this book, from a post-Prosodic Morphology perspective, is the section on "Deriving Reduplicative Templates", where Raimy abandons all notions of prosody in favor of segmental X slots. He uses data from Tohono O'odham for light syllable reduplication, where he uses "jump links", which promote skipping of segments, to derive the effects of vowel reduction in the base, and data from Agta, Ilokano, and Mokilese to argue for X-slot insertion to derive heavy syllable reduplicants (or not, in the case of Kusaiean). A-templatic reduplication is covered through the use of jump links, such as "from the first segment of the string to the last segment, then to the first segment preceding a vowel" in Temiar (slog -> s-g- log), and "first segment of the string to the last segment, then back to the first segment" in Semai (c?e:t -> ct-c?e:t) (these are my paraphrases of his rules).
While I grant that such rules can be invoked to derive many reduplicative templates, Raimy does not discuss any cases where reduplicative "bases" (to use the OT terminology; Raimy does not even have "bases", which is also a problem for him, as we will see presently) are themselves comprised of prosodic units. McCarthy and Prince (1986) cite the example of Yidiny, wherein the first foot of the base is fully copied, and no more. For example, /kintalpa/ surfaces as [kintal.kintalpa], but /mulari/ surfaces as [mula.mulari] and not [*mular.mulari]. Similarly, Yaqui verbs (in the default syllabic case) only copy the first syllable of the root: /chepta/ yields [chep.chepta], but /vusa/ yields [vu.vusa], not [*vus.vusa]. In these cases (among others), the most perspicuous explanation for what is occurring is not that what is being copied is a series of C's and V's (e.g. "copy up to the first V in CVCV forms, and up to the second C in CVCCV forms"), but that what is being copied are specific prosodic constituents of the root.
Chapter 4 provides a summary to the book, and presents what Raimy sees as the biggest problems with Optimality Theory: conspiracies, over-predicting possible reduplication patterns (e.g. the "Kager-Hamilton problem", in which a highly-ranked MAX-BR leads to alteration of the base to conform to the shape of the reduplicant), and reduplication-specific mechanisms, such as the identity relation between reduplicant and base.
CRITICAL EVALUATION
One of the most appealing aspects of Raimy's proposal is his overt articulation of the structure received by the phonological component, which I will concede is often neglected in Optimality Theory. Raimy argues specifically for concatenation of morphemes, and in Chapter 3 he proposes a series of precedence variables to derive such notions as prefix, suffix, infix, reduplicant, and free morpheme, and uses his linearization technique to derive such forms as English "boyishness" and "table Smable".
Although I share Raimy's theoretical predisposition for a modular Morphological Structure, as in DM, I do not think that such a view is incompatible with Optimality Theory. Here I will address two of Raimy's principal arguments against OT: the status of RED as a morpheme and the ability of OT to render phonological explanations.
Raimy has two principle objections to an abstract morpheme RED. The first has to do with the problem of OT allowing multiple reduplicants in a specific language. Raimy reasons thus: "if a reduplication pattern is only and truly a derivative of language-specific features then there should be no possibility of multiple patterns of reduplication in a single language" (pp. 96-7). This might be true, if we grant the assumption that RED is not a morpheme. Since OT does not have this assumption, (under the assumptions of Generalized Alignment) alignment constraints can freely refer to variable reduplicant shapes. For example, a hypothetical language might have a single syllable reduplicant for habitual action in verbs, and a foot reduplicant for plurals in nouns, and both of these notions could straightforwardly be accounted for with alignment constraints: Align (habitual, L, syllable, L) and Align (plural, L, foot, L).
His second objection to a RED-as-morpheme approach is that "the Optimality Theory approach that claims that RED has no phonological content and then derives the surface phonology of the reduplicative morpheme through the interaction of constraints denies the morphological nature of the affix in question" (p.97). I am not quite sure what he could mean by this, since it is his own theory which claims that the reduplicant is not an affix, thus is non- morphological, and it is OT which actively asserts what he claims that it denies.
Raimy's primary motivation for proposing a new theory of reduplication is the effort to get rid of reduplication- specific mechanisms (correspondence), such as Anchor and BR- Faithfulness. It is true that his model does this. However, throughout the book Raimy trumpets the fact that his theory is capable of accounting for the various phenomena that he discusses. The question that kept raising in this reviewer's mind is this: What can he not do? Recall the rule from above: "link from the last consonant to the segment preceding the first vowel". With such computational power, in principle anything can be done, and this is not surprising given the generative power of rule-based systems. Raimy does appeal to the notion of "analytic simplicity" to favor relatively few links, but this notion does not seem to apply to the complexity of the generalization of the links that he needs to get the results that he achieves.
Finally, I must address one final argument that Raimy lodges toward OT: the charge that OT is a "conspiracy". Here, I quote him at length:
It is the entire ranking of all constraints that evaluate a reduplication structure that accounts for the reduplication pattern and this makes it unclear as to what generalization is provided by a single constraint that affects some dimension of reduplication. Due to this fact, OT analyses of reduplication are open to conspiracy arguments since the generalization accounting for the pattern does not result from a single statement in the grammar. Instead, generalizations with respect to reduplication in OT are provided by the entire grammar of a given language (p.160).
It seems that what is at issue here is what constitutes a grammar. Raimy apparently wants a specific statement for each phonological generalization in the language. On the other hand, OT provides a single generalization for the entire language: a list of ranked constraints. It is not clear to me why the former is superior to the latter, since the latter captures more information (an entire grammar) with fewer statements than the former. Ultimately, this issue, along with other such issues as whether or not there is utility to such notions as prosodic units, etc., will have to be left to the reader to decide.
At the conclusion of his book, Raimy suggests possible extensions of his model into other areas of morphology, particularly templatic morphology and subtractive morphology. Both of these morphological phenomena employ the same mechanism in his system as reduplication: namely, "jump links", wherein, for example in the latter case, a 0- morpheme triggers a phonological rewrite rule which adds an arrow from a particular segment to '%', the end of the string. This unified account is a nice result for DM, since phonological material is never actually "erased" (which is a theoretical impossibility in DM) but instead fails to get parsed because of the added link. (This is similar to the Containment model of Prince and Smolensky (1993)). For theorists who object to 0-morphemes, however, the entire model will fail to be convincing since it is central to his theory (and to DM).
Summary
In sum, this brief book is well worth reading, and does provide an improved derivational model for reduplication. While I do not think that the arguments laid forth herein will sway anybody to defect from their current position on the derivational/optimality debate, this book does serve as a very useful model for derivationalists and Distributed Morphologists, and is particularly useful as a problem book for people working in Optimality Theory. Some of the issues that Raimy raises about Optimality Theory require rebuttal, and this kind of critical appraisal of the widely-accepted (and perhaps currently hegemonic) theory about language is welcome. After all, we are all looking for the best theory, and alternative theories, such as the proposal made in this book by Raimy, usefully serve to keep us working in that direction.
References
Haugen, J. 2000. Distributing morphology in Yaqui "secondary" reduplication. Ms, Department of Linguistics, University of Arizona, Tucson.
McCarthy, J. and A. Prince. 1986. Prosodic morphology. Ms. Published as Technical Report #32, Center for Cognitive Sciences, Rutgers University.
McCarthy, J. and A. Prince. 1995. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In University of Massachusetts occasional papers in linguistics 18, 249-384.GLSA,University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Spaelti, P. 1997. Dimensions of variation in multi-pattern reduplication. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Linguistics, University of California, Santa Cruz
About the reviewer:
Jason Haugen is a third-year graduate student in the joint Ph.D. program in Linguistics and Anthropology at the University of Arizona. His primary interests are in Native American linguistics, with particular focus on the Uto- Aztecan languages.
|