Date: Thu, 23 Dec 2004 14:08:25 -0600 From: Larry LaFond Subject: The Second Glot International State-of-the-Article Book
EDITORS: Cheng, Lisa; Sybesma, Rint TITLE: The Second Glot International State-of-the-Article Book SUBTITLE: The Latest in Linguistics SERIES: Studies in Generative Grammar 61 PUBLISHER: Mouton de Gruyter YEAR: 2002
Larry L. LaFond, Southern Illinois University Edwardsville
INTRODUCTION
This volume brings together fifteen articles that provide an overview of some of the most important linguistic developments during the last decade. The intent of each paper is to capture the prominent themes and issues related to its area of focus, describe progress that has been made, indicate questions still to be answered, and supply an extensive bibliography for those interested in further study. Earlier versions of each of the papers in this volume appeared in GLOT INTERNATIONAL as 'State- of-the-Article' contributions, although each of those earlier versions were updated and revised to include further developments since the time of their original publication. The papers in this volume assume a generative perspective and focus on semantics and syntax, though a few papers are also included that relate to phonology, morphology and first language development. The papers stand on their own, without any introduction or framing by the editors.
SYNOPSIS
Lightfoot's 'The development of grammars' opens the series of articles. Lightfoot first considers the nature of the experience that triggers the development of grammars in children, discussing several recent error- driven models (where learners converge on a grammar that matches up with environmental input, within a space defined by UG) and how cue-based theories differ from those input matching models. Lightfoot then looks at extensions of the cue-based acquisition approach to diachronic change in grammars. In the process, he argues for cue-based approaches to acquisition and against lexicalist theories of grammar, an independent theory of change, and the incorporation of historicist elements into UG.
Two papers on semantics follow Lightfoot's paper: Authier's 'Semantics and the Generative Enterprise' and Portner's 'The semantics of Mood.' Authier, through an examination of quantifier scope, bare output conditions, and the mapping from LF to logical representations, argues against the notion that generative grammar has little to say about meaning-related phenomena. Authier suggests that while the range of semantic phenomena that have been examined within a generative framework only partially overlaps with the concerns of model-theoretic semanticists, the phenomena which have been studied have yielded important issues and conditions for interpretation, enough so that a more clearly articulated theory of the syntax-semantics interface is needed. Portner discusses the core phenomenon of mood as viewed within traditional grammar and then expands the discussion to other senses of the term. He outlines some of the central data that research on mood has attempted to address: the distribution of mood in root clauses, the distribution of subjunctives in embedded clauses, and embedded indicatives. He then outlines the major differences between theoretical analyses and concludes with a summary of future directions.
Portner is followed by de Swart's 'Three approaches to discourse and donkey anaphora' and a second paper related to quantifiers, Bobaljik's 'Floating quantifiers: Handle with care.' De Swart compares the option of saying that the anaphoric pronoun in donkey anaphora cannot be interpreted in terms of regular coreference or binding with two other options: one, that interprets indefinite NPs as variables and use unselective binding to allow them to be bound by other quantifiers in the discourse, and another, that views the licensing problem as a problem with the definition of the binding domain. Bobaljik's discussion provides historical perspective on floating quantifiers up through the 1980s as a background to discussing work done since then on stranding and the semantics of floating quantifiers. Bobaljik concludes that caution should be used in regarding floating quantifiers as tests for underlying constituent structure, but that they are connected to predication and that their distribution is attributable to movement and/or binding.
Carlson's 'No lack of determination,' de Hoop's 'Partitivity', and Szabolcsi and den Dikken's 'Islands' present additional areas of syntactic and semantic interfaces. Carlson focuses on interpretations of Bare Plurals, looking specifically at the question of whether Bare Plurals have a single, unified meaning (one which appears to be different in different contexts), or whether they have more than one unified meaning. Carlson concludes that general agreement exists that existential and generic readings of Bare Plurals should find some commonality in analysis, and that specificity and scope are important issues for indefinite readings. Beyond this, however, Carlson sees a 'bewildering variety of proposals' related to the source of existential quantification in some Bare Plurals. Partitive elements also make certain sets or entities accessible for quantification; so argues de Hoop in her article which describes the similarities between different types of partitivity. De Hoop sees a distinction between ordinary partitives (where quantification involves restricted or contextually bound sets) and pseudo-partitives, faded partitives, and partitive Case (where the set available for quantification is unrestricted or unbounded.) Finally, Szabolcsi and den Dikken's article on 'Islands' presents a brief discussion of strong islands and a more extensive discussion of weak islands, highlighting how the advent of Relativized Minimality has affected theories in this area. The authors believe that many island conditions are semantic in nature and that the semantic approaches that currently hold the most promises are algebraic and dynamic versions of Scope Theory.
Four papers on syntax, albeit from widely different perspectives follow. The first, Progovac's 'Structure for coordination' divides nicely into three parts: a section introducing coordination data, one that surveys analyses of coordination that do not treat conjunctions as heads of conjunction phrases, and one that surveys analyses that do. Progovac concludes that conjunctions are functional heads of Coordination Phrases, that in VO languages the first conjunct stands structurally apart from the rest of the Coordination Phrase, and that the conjunction and the non- initial conjuncts form a structural unit. Müller's 'Optionality in optimality-theoretic syntax' takes on a perplexing issue for most syntactic theories: syntactic optionality. In the process, Müller not only succeeds in showing various optimality-theoretic approaches to optionality (pseudo-optionality, ordered global or local ties, conjunctive or disjunctive ties, neutralization, etc.), but also provides a description of optimality-theoretic approaches to syntax as well as what challenges optionality continues to pose for those working within this framework. Rosen's 'The syntactic representation of linguistic events' discusses differing approaches to representing the relationship between events in the world and how these events become encoded in language. Rosen's paper summarizes a large body of research aimed at event classification, discusses three main theoretical approaches to event representation -- lexical, semantic, syntactic -- and then indicates major unanswered questions for this field. Rosen concludes that events are represented, in some way, in all three of the above components of the grammar, but that relationship between the components has yet to be successfully explained.
Manzini's 'Syntactic approaches to cliticization,' serves as the final syntax-related paper and the first of three papers related to phonological theory, and as bridge between the two fields. Although clitics are a prosodic conception, the survey provided by Manzini adopts a syntactic perspective. Beginning with Kayne's (1975) view of cliticization as a movement rule, Manzini then reviews developments in the 70s and 80s, and later analyses of clitics as inflectional heads, concluding that the current need is for a clear structural map of clitic positions and the parameters that account for them. Manzini also includes a discussion of morphological and prosodic treatments of clitics.
Two final papers on phonology, Rice's 'Featural markedness in phonology: variation' and van Oostendorp's 'Schwa in phonological theory' deal, respectively, with general and specific phonological issues. Rice queries how 'markedness' might be defined and what role markedness plays in a phonological system. Rice discusses feature specification and feature classes, phonological diagnostics for markedness, variation in markedness (related to position, inventories, contrast, and phonetic space), and traditional diagnostics for markedness, frequency and implication. Rice also includes a section which nicely exemplifies markedness in structural theories, Optimality Theory, and phonetic cue-based theories. A more specific issue is taken up by van Oostendorp's article that considers schwa as an excellent test case for phonological theories. Three types of schwa are defined and considered by van Oostendorp: one alternating with zero (epenthetic-schwa), one alternating with a full vowel (reduction- schwa), and a 'rest' case (stable-schwa). Van Oostendorp discusses the representation and behavior of each of these schwas, concluding that a fully developed theory of syllable structure, metrical structure, segmental structure and the relationship between these parts is still needed.
Harley and Noyer's 'Distributed Morphology' is the final contribution of the book. A few of the earlier articles included brief discussion of interfaces between morphology and other components of grammar, but this article is the only one focused specifically on morphosyntax. Throughout their paper, Harley and Noyer provide some review of the substantial body of literature that has arisen since Distributed Morphology (Halle and Marantz 1993) made its appearance in the early 1990s, however they concentrate their efforts on a presentation of Distributed Morphology's theoretical assumptions, its architecture, and illustrations of its implementation. They attempt to show Distributed Morphology's contributions to grammatical theory and provide a solid introduction to the work that is going on within Distributed Morphology. Harley and Noyer suggest that, going forward, the most important issues for this theory of grammar will relate to a reassessment of the inventory and bases for syntactic categories, a clearer description of universal morphosyntactic features, and a number of remaining questions related to Distributed Morphology's novel operations: impoverishment, fission, and morphological merger.
EVALUATION
Any volume which purports to review the most important achievements of the past decade will be subject to criticism by those who disagree with its conceptualization of what the most important issues or most pressing research questions are. The last decade has seen significant work done in a great number of linguistic areas that do not find their way into this volume on the 'latest in linguistics.' This is, perhaps, a forgivable transgression, given that no single volume could be expected to cover the full breadth of linguistic issues during an era, and particularly given that the focus of this series is on generative approaches to grammar. Nevertheless, one might reasonably expect a more extensive discussion of minimalism, generative approaches to second language acquisition, recent developments in construction grammar, or the inclusion of a broader range of issues that arise within generative grammar. Future volumes of this nature could consider the possibility of not including multiple articles on semantics, phonology, or any other single area of linguistics, if the presence of multiple articles on a single area comes only at the expense of exclusion of a broader range of current topics in linguistics. That the current volume leans most heavily towards semantics and interfaces between semantics and syntax will satisfy only a particular readership.
It is an interesting feature of the book that there is no introduction or preview to frame the articles, no biographical information given about the authors, few acknowledgements, and virtually no foot or endnotes. Some might view these as failings, but presented as it is, the book gives the sense of presenting just the ideas, the questions, and references in an uncluttered and useful way. I found it particularly useful that some authors (for example, Bobaljik, Harley and Noyer) separated the specific bibliography for their area of focus from other references that happened to be mentioned in their papers. This provides a solid, targeted reading list for students or researchers working within the area of interest.
Some of the papers are more successful than others in helping non- specialists get a feel for the kind of work that is being done in a specific area. For example, the papers by Lightfoot, Portner, Müller, and Rice provide excellent, readable introductions to the issues in their respective fields. I would not hesitate to steer students to these papers to get a broad introduction to those research areas. Some of the other papers, while still very complete in presenting current issues, are less accessible to those who have not had formal training in that area. For example, fully understanding de Swart's discussion of dynamic binding requires more than a rudimentary knowledge of propositional logic. Nevertheless, the bibliographies at the end of each of the articles make the volume invaluable, in fact, this feature alone makes the book useful for researchers and graduate students exploring the issues raised in this volume.
In general, this book provides a good overview of the state of (some areas) of linguistic inquiry at the start of this millennium. Readers will encounter interesting findings and ample evidence that linguistic research during the last decade has uncovered many of the questions yet to be answered. As nearly all of the authors in this volume point out, there is a still a need to delve more deeply into the host of unanswered questions. Each article here, probably appropriately, succeeds in being more a call to future work than a solution to a current problem.
REFERENCES
Halle, M and H. Marantz. (1993). Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In: The View from the Building 20, K. Hale and S. J. Keyser (eds.), 111-176. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Kayne, R. (1975). French Syntax: The transformational cycle. Cambridge: MIT Press.
|