Date: Wed, 13 Jul 2005 14:06:50 +0300 From: Marius Nagy Subject: Approaches to Cognition through Text and Discourse
EDITOR: Virtanen, Tuija TITLE: Approaches to Cognition through Text and Discourse PUBLISHER: Mouton de Gruyter YEAR: 2004
Jozsef Marius Nagy, François-Rabelais University
INTRODUCTION
Recent developments in discourse linguistics as well as in cognitive linguistics have highlighted the common interest of these approaches of language. But, despite their share interest in issues of discourse and cognition, there is a wide gap between them caused by differences in the frameworks and perspectives adopted for study.
The aim of the volume edited by Tuija Virtanen is to contribute to bridging that gap by an interdisciplinary debate. The volume is organised in individual chapters which, despite the variety of methods adopted for study, focus on text and discourse in a given situational context and on individual and distributed cognition. As Virtanen points out in the introductory chapter "what is shared is an awareness for the fact that discourse and cognition can only be studied with the help of discourse and cognition". The individual contributions deal with a large area of data from a several languages (including discourse in bilingual settings) ranging from narrative to non-narrative, spoken to written, informative to literary, experimental to authentic, professional genres to impromptu speech, from public to private or semi-private discourse.
DESCRIPTION
In the opening chapter (Text, discourse and cognition: An introduction), Tuija Virtanen discusses the relation between language, discourse and cognition. Text and discourse linguistics focus on text/discourse in context. Virtanen highlights the bi-directional view of text and context: if the form of a particular text is affected by its context, texts and discourses themselves contribute to the construction, maintaining and alteration of contexts. In cognitive linguistics, much studies dealing with text and discourse has been centred on individual cognition, i.e. "the inferences that people are required to make to interpret discourse, and the assumptions they seem to be making about their interlocutors' consciousness and memory constraints as manifested in discourse". The author argues convincingly for a shift of interest to 'distributed cognition'. A central area in the study of discourse and cognition is the analysis of the variation across texts and discourses. In Virtanen's opinion, there are two essential dimensions of discourse variation: text types and genres, both notions being prototypical categorizations. Text types "reflect the way in which we view reality". As she did earlier (cf. Virtanen 1992), the author argues for a distinction between two basic, idealised, text types: the narrative, who "involves human beings in a dynamic series of actions that have an outcome of some sort different from the situation at the beginning of the series", and the non-narrative (argumentative). Genres are closely related to particular socio-cultural contexts. They are part of our distributed cognition or the shared knowledge of a discourse community.
In chapter 2, entitled "Language, discourse and cognition: retrospect and prospects", Robert de Beaugrande presents a dialectical model of language and discourse such that language, conceived as "a theory of human knowledge and experience", "specifies the standing constraints (i.e. what words usually mean), whereas discourse, conceived as practice of human knowledge and experience, "manifests emergent constraints (i.e. what the words mean in this particular stretch of discourse)". Hence, theory of language turns into 'theoretical practice', essentially 'practice-driven', and practice of discourse into 'practical theory', essentially 'theory driven'. Similarly, 'cognition' and 'language' interact in a dialectical cycle, such that "cognition generates meanings, whereas language determines meanings". Meanings are conceived not as mere units, but as events in "a dialectical process which always has a context as its cognitive architecture". A great challenge for cognitive linguistics, and a possible way to bridge the gap between text linguistics and cognitive linguistics, is the study of meaning by examining a "very large corpora of text and discourse". In the final of his contribution, the author argues in favour of what he calls "cognitive text linguistics".
In her paper (On the discourse basis of person agreement), Anna Siewierska examines person agreement making cross-linguistically. Hence, she critically discusses two diachronic scenarios that have been proposed in the literature for its development. The first, proposed by T. Givon, also called NP-detachment, postulates that person agreement markers originate in the third person pronouns. In the second, elaborated by Mira Ariel on the basis of accessibility theory, the gramaticalization of person agreement markers originates in the first and second person pronouns. Siewierska argues for an extension of Ariel's model to third person forms as well as to object functions too.
In chapter 4, entitled 'The information structure of bilingual meaning: a constructivist approach to Californian Finnish conversation', M. M. Jocelyne Fernandez-Vest examines information structure in impromptu speech emerging in both monolingual and bilingual contexts, using the model of Theme-Rheme-Mneme. The third element, Mneme (close to the 'tail' of Functional Grammar or to Lambrecht's Construction Grammar 'antitopic'), refers to formal properties ( a flat intonation) and semantic ones ( supposedly shared knowledge, affective modulation). The analysis of the Californian Finnish narratives shows that there is a tendency for the Rheme or the Mneme to be marked by code-switching from Finnish to English. In conversation, code-switching is motivated b situational needs (e.g. the presence of a monolingual addressee or the intention to be exact about referents related to life in the USA) or the interlocutors 'memory processes' (e.g. memorized social situation). The chapter deals also with the problem of quantitative memory exemplified on the basis of a monolingual (Sami) and a bilingual (Finnish Californian) corpus. In Sami contexts, men and women manifest different mechanism of remembering dates, estimating distances. In Californian Finnish contexts, the sex differences concern code-switching at temporal signposts of a story.
Chapter 5 (Point of departure: cognitive aspects of sentence-initial adverbials) explores the discourse functions of sentence-initial adverbials (i.e. adverbs, prepositional phrases, noun phrases, phrases and clauses) in written texts from a cognitive point of view. Virtanen focuses her analysis on the "prototypical adverbials of time, place and manner". The analysis shows that sentence-initial position has a great cognitive potential, creating coherence and determining point of view for what follows. Particular categories of adverbials are 'professionalized' in given discourse functions indicating manner of speaking, beliefs, attitudes and so forth. Finally, sentence-initial adverbials can be related to distributed knowledge as they can be connected to the socio- cultural context.
Chapter 6 entitled 'What is foregrounded in narratives. Hypothesis for the cognitive basis of foregrounding' is by Brita Wårwik. The author explores the parallels between the textual foreground-background distinction and perceptual and cognitive principles of organization. The foregrounding and backgrounding of elements in narratives is analysed by three cognitive perspectives: the figure-ground distinction, the EVENT prototype and salience, including also a consideration of the role of iconicity.
The contribution of Lita Lundquist (From legal knowledge to legal discourse and back again) deals with the analysis of expert and non-expert knowledge of two types of legal concepts: 'contract' and 'judgement'. Using the semantic notion of 'qualia' borrowed from Pustejovsky's works (cf. The Generative Lexical, 1995), the author shows the great difference that exists between the knowledge structure of expert and non-expert text. The analysis highlights that experts not only use a higher number of qualia than non-experts but they are also able to recognize more qualia than a non-expert one.
In chapter 8 Anne Marie Bülow-Møller (Conditionals: your spaces or mine) proposes a study of conditionals in context arguing that classifications based on decontextualized sentences do not hold in practice. The analysis shows that use of conditionals in argumentative discourse cannot be accounted for without a consideration of the context and the communicative strategies of the interlocutor engaged in the discourse.
The paper of Martina Björklund (Communicative fragments and the interpretation of discourse) focuses on the analysis of literary discourse. The author argues, following Gasparov, the essential role of the recognition of communicative fragments with their mental representations and the whole communicative landscape they evoke (genres, styles, thematic fields, concrete texts and utterances) in the perception/interpretation of discourse, of any discourse not just literary one. In the conception of Gasparov, communicative fragments are "discourse fragments of various lengths, stored n the speakers' memory as whole fixed elements, i.e. they are not generated by the speaker according to grammatical rules". Communicative fragments are given in a dynamic way such that is by principle impossible to say where one communicative fragment ends and where another begins.
In the final chapter of the volume (Drawing the line: a contested conceptual model in Danish 'child care talk'), Peter Harder investigates the negotiation of common ground, through the analysis of a particular conceptual model in a Danish context, that of 'drawing the line' (grœnse metaphor) in the interaction between adults and children from whom they are responsible. The metaphor is used about situations where the issue comes up whether adult should restrain the children's activity or not. The author highlights the role of mapping from source to target domain "as a social process rather than a purely conceptual one" and argues for the conception of conceptualization as an ongoing process "where mental structures meets actual experience and there is a struggle to impose some conceptual order in it" rather than as a product, i.e. "as a part of a fully mapped-out conceptual world".
CRITICAL EVALUATION
The volume represents an important contribution to the development of both text/discourse linguistics and cognitive linguistics. The emphasis on 'distributed cognition' in several articles makes possible the focus on the cultural dimension of cognitive sciences and the elaboration of a semiotics of culture. This fact implies the focus on "the fundamental character of creation inherent to the cognitive essence of language" (Coseriu, 1952/1991), which, unfortunately, lack in many works done in the area of cognitive linguistics. Hence, I completely agree with the critical remarks raised against cognitive semantics by Coseriu (1990). A text, as Rastier (2001) points out, is not a set of cognitive schemata. Its structure doesn't consist in mental correlates. A text is not a set of representations, but a structured set of constraints on the formation of representations.
REFERENCES
Borcila, Mircea, 1997, The Metaphoric Model in Poetic Texts, in Szoveg és stilus. Text si stil. Text and Style, Cluj, Presa Universitara, p. 97-104
Coseriu, Eugenio, 1952/1991, La creacion metaforica en el lenguaje, in E. Coseriu, El hombre y su lenguaje. Estudios de teoria y metodologia lingüistica, Madrid, Gredos, p. 66-102
Coseriu, Eugenio, 1980/1997, Linguistica del testo. Introduzione a una ermeneutica del senso, Roma, La Nuova Italia Scientifica
Coseriu, Eugenio, 1990, Semantica estructural y semantica cognitiva, in "Jornadas de filologia", Barcelona, pp. 239-282
Rastier, François, 2001, Arts et sciences du texte, Paris, PUF
|