Author: Christopher J Hall Title: An Introduction to Language and Linguistics Subtitle: Breaking the Language Spell Published: 2005 Publisher: Continuum International Publishing Group Ltd.
Reviewed by John Fry, San José State University
Christopher J. Hall has written a brief but remarkable new introduction to language and linguistics. The book's central theme is the ''Language Spell'', a metaphor coined by Hall to describe the ''evolutionary magic'' that prevents us from seeing how language really works even as we unconsciously take it for granted in our daily lives. The field of linguistics is presented as the scientific quest to break out of the spell and to glimpse the hidden structure underlying our language use.
SUMMARY
The book is divided into four parts. Part I, entitled ''Magic'', introduces Hall's metaphor of the Language Spell. ''In our daily lives,'' writes Hall, ''we operate as though under a spell, content to know that language is there, but not able to see it plainly or penetrate is mystery... The language spell keeps most of the extraordinary nature of human language tidily in the background as we concentrate on the messages it conveys.''
Hall then introduces his second theme, the Fundamental Paradox. The Fundamental Paradox of language is that it is at the same time a biological and a sociocultural entity. That is, while languages exist only in individual human minds, they only work if they are perceived as shared by social groups. Because the biological reality of language is hidden by the Spell, we are normally aware of only its social purposes.
The remainder of the book introduces the reader to a broad variety of topics in the fields of theoretical and applied linguistics, demonstrating the different ways that linguists go about ''bombarding language with the counter-spells of science''.
Part II, entitled ''Words'', addresses what words are, how they arise, and how they relate to concepts in the minds of speakers and hearers. Each lexical item, Hall explains, is represented in the mind as a 'triad' composed of a physically externalizable form (phonological, orthographic, or signed); a syntactic frame; and a meaning. Other topics introduced here include language change, the phonological structure of words, and children's acquisition of word meaning.
Part III, ''Grammar'', covers morphology, syntax, language acquisition, and pragmatics. Here Hall sketches an account of how language-neutral conceptual structures (including thematic roles) are mapped onto linguistic expressions via the syntactic component of the language faculty. The book's treatment of lexical and syntactic theory in Parts II and III is strongly influenced by the ideas of Ray Jackendoff (2002), as Hall is eager to acknowledge.
Finally, Part IV, ''Babel'', addresses variation, both between languages (typology) and within languages (sociolinguistics), before concluding with a section entitled ''Living under the Language Spell''. Here Hall wraps up his discussion of the Fundamental Paradox. Language, he concludes, ''is simultaneously both biological and sociocultural, because that's it's job: to link separate biological organisms through a channel which allows them to share thoughts and feelings, and so build an individual identity that is also integrated into a series of culturally-defined groups.'' Hall ends the book with an appeal for wider dissemination of linguistic knowledge as a way of combatting language prejudice, language death, and other consequences of linguistic ignorance.
Throughout the text, Hall illustrates his points with constructed examples as well as with snippets of language use taken from novels, telephone transcripts, court proceedings, and other examples from everyday life.
One striking feature of the book is how seriously it takes written language. Throughout the text, writing is presented as a modality of language on par with speaking and signing. This contrasts with the ''phonocentric'' stance often associated with modern linguistics.
EVALUATION
There are already dozens of introductory texts on language and linguistics. Do we really need another one?
In this case, the answer is ''yes''. Hall's book is highly original, thanks mainly to the ''breaking the spell'' trope that underlies and motivates it. As Hall explains in the Preface, ''I have written this book in a way that embraces and integrates the social and the psychological aspects of language, using the spell metaphor to bridge the gap.'' At that he has clearly succeeded.
Hall is enthusiastic about his subject and explains it well. His prose is vivid and colorful and packed with apt metaphors. Some examples: ''Activation of elements in memory is not like an on/off switch, but more like the warming up and cooling down of an oven'' Hall writes about lexical priming. Later, on innateness: ''The brain of a newborn child or chimp is not like the hollow shell of a new building, with empty rooms awaiting plumbing and power, furniture and fittings. Instead it is already cabled for electricity and hooked up to the Internet... just waiting for the interior decorators...'' Hall's prose does occasionally fall flat however: ''a new sentence is more commonplace than a sneeze during the Moscow winter''.
Succinctness and brevity are among the book's virtues, but in many places the discussion simply zips by too fast. Hall warns the reader about the breakneck pace in the preface: ''What you'll experience in the following chapters is a roller-coaster ride through the labyrinth of human language.'' I sometimes found myself wishing that Hall would slow the coaster down, and some readers may be tempted to jump off. Some important topics blaze by so quickly that you miss them if you blink. Most glaringly, phonetics and phonology are relegated to just under four pages (!), in a subsection entitled ''Sounds and phonemes'' tucked deep inside Chapter 5. Many of the topics that Hall rushes through cry out for a helpful illustrative example or anecdote. For instance, the explanation of the contrast in language production of Broca's and Wernicke's aphasics in Chapter 11 could have been nicely complemented with some short representative transcriptions of aphasic speech.
A number of topics that one normally finds in an introductory text are missing from Hall's book. My own pet peeve in this regard is the omission of count nouns and mass nouns (although Hall does distinguish ''concrete'' from ''abstract'' nouns). The syntax chapter introduces the notions of phrase-structure rules and X-bar structure, but (wisely in my view) does not wade into the swamp of transformations and empty categories.
The sections on linguistic bigotry and discrimination are generally even-handed, although American readers might quibble with Hall's characterization of the USA as an ''officially monolingual nation'' (p. 223) where ''Latin American immigrants...have had their traditional languages taken away from them'' (p. 25).
The book is written in a very informal style that is often refreshing but sometimes off-putting. For example, Hall frequently uses dot-dot-dot ellipsis in the main text to indicate pause or hesitation rather than omission (e.g., ''dreams ... and so much more'', p. 51). These ellipses give the text a chatty feel that seems out of place in an academic work. I sometimes found myself longing for the iron-fist editing and attention to typesetting detail associated with traditional textbooks, with their em-dashes and serial commas.
The informality of the book also extends to its cross references. In Chapter 11, near the end of the book, the reader is referred back to ''the egg diagram'' in Chapter 3. Navigating my way back to this diagram, some 200 pages earlier in the text, proved quite challenging. The book offers no list of figures. Since the page headers do not indicate chapter numbers, I had to consult the table of contents in order to find my way back to Chapter 3. Next, I had to flip through all of Chapter 3 to find the figures (there are four) and then determine which of these was the ''egg diagram'' (the figure's title does not include these words). A helpful cross reference to ''page 67'' would have been appreciated.
Hall states plainly in the preface that the book is not a conventional textbook. ''It's too selective and idiosyncratic in coverage,'' Hall explains, ''to provide the kind of rigorous survey needed by students in linguistics courses.'' Instead, Hall states, ''The purpose of the book is to appeal to as many ordinary readers as possible, to share with them a broad vision of the wonders of human language and the peril of taking them for granted.''
I agree that this is not a linguistics textbook, but that is largely to its credit. Compared to Hall's book, most linguistics textbooks are bloated (not to mention scandalously expensive) and seem to be (and often are) written by committees. This book is pleasantly idiosyncratic but always focused on the big picture: what language is and how it works.
On the other hand, there are two features that good linguistics textbooks offer that Hall's book doesn't. The first is data from a variety of languages. Hall's examples are almost exclusively in English, with a smattering of Spanish. The second feature missing from Hall's book is a set of exercises at end of each chapter. In my opinion, both elements, diverse language data and problem sets, are essential components of any introductory linguistics course. Supplemented by these resources, Hall's engaging book would make a fine textbook for an introductory course, even though it was not intended as such.
REFERENCES
Jackendoff, Ray. Foundations of Language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford University Press, 2002.
|