LINGUIST List 36.2714
Fri Sep 12 2025
Calls: Workshop at SLE 2026: Discourse Coherence and Clausal Complementation: Diachronic Pathways and Diagnostic Problems (Germany)
Editor for this issue: Valeriia Vyshnevetska <valeriialinguistlist.org>
Date: 09-Sep-2025
From: Haiping Long <lhpszpt126.com>
Subject: Workshop at SLE 2026: Discourse Coherence and Clausal Complementation: Diachronic Pathways and Diagnostic Problems
E-mail this message to a friend
Full Title: Workshop at SLE 2026: Discourse Coherence and Clausal Complementation: Diachronic Pathways and Diagnostic Problems
Short Title: SLE2026
Date: 26-Aug-2026 - 29-Aug-2026
Location: Osnabrück, Germany
Contact Person: Anna Kisiel
Meeting Email: [email protected]
Web Site: https://societaslinguistica.eu/sle2026/
Linguistic Field(s): Applied Linguistics; General Linguistics; Linguistic Theories; Philosophy of Language
Call Deadline: 10-Nov-2025
Call for Papers:
Björn Wiemer, 2 Haiping Long, 3 Giulia Mazzola
1 JGU Mainz ([email protected]),
2 Sun Yat-sen University ([email protected]),
3 Newcastle University ([email protected])
Until now, research on the pragmatics-syntax interface of discourse, on the one hand, and on clausal complementation, on the other, have been developing rather independently. This workshop aims at bringing these two strands together, with a focus on delimitation problems between asyndetic complementation, quotation and parenthetical comment clauses as well as their diachronic relations.
Clausal complementation consists in “biclausal syntactic constructions in which the predicate of one clause ‘entails reference to another proposition or state of affairs’ (Cristofaro 2003: 95), expressed in a second clause” (Schmidtke-Bode 2014: 7). In turn, discourse coherence arises from continuing parts of meaning across clauses. This implies that meaning components overlap, or are compatible (cf. Das/Taboada 2018, Haselow/Hancil 2021), regardless of how clauses connect: by mere juxtaposition (1a), by a connective explicitly naming the relation (because in 1b) or by one marking a subsequent clause as complement (that in 1c).
(1a) Peter was upset, they forgot his coffee.
(1b) Peter was upset because they forgot his coffee.
(1c) Peter was upset that they forgot his coffee.
Consequently, clausal complementation represents just a specific case of coherence between adjacent clauses: the complement clause supplies ‘content’ which complies, or is compatible, with the semantic potential of a complement-taking predicate (CTP). The latter may be represented by verbs, nouns, adjectives or other parts of speech, while the content may represent states of affairs or propositions.
It seems natural to posit diachronic links between looser types of discourse-coherence and clausal complementation, and a synchronic gradient between them. A wealth of publications has addressed the “loose end” of discourse coherence, e.g. the emergence of parenthetical predicates (e.g., Thompson/Mulac 1991, Vazquez Rozas 2006, Brinton 2008, Heine 2013, Heine et al. 2021), but also the development of complementation from ‘complementation strategies’ (Dixon 2006; e.g., Arsenijević 2009, Schmidtke-Bode 2009: 157-165, Sonnenhauser 2015, Meyer 2017, Grković-Major 2021) and the relation between parentheticals and complementation (e.g., Schneider 2007: 177–184, Boye/Harder 2021, Mazzola 2022: 61-65). Yet, the diachronic conditions and processes leading from discourse coherence strategies to complementation (or vice versa) are still poorly understood. Moreover, proposed diagnostics often do not lead to clear decisions. For instance, the criterion of relative discourse prominence (Boye/Harder 2021) cannot discriminate cases like (1a) or (2): if the first clause is discourse secondary, it may be a parenthetical comment or a matrix clause plus asyndetic complement.
Clause 1 Clause 2
(2) I think you are wrong.
We furthermore encounter competing views concerning the pathways along which clausal complementation may arise. Consider, in particular, the expansion and integration pathways in Heine/Kuteva (2007: 216–251):
(3) a. Clause [NP] → Clause 1 [Clause 2] Expansion
b. Clause 1 + Clause 2 → Clause 1 [Clause 2] Integration
Expansion corresponds to analogy and integration to reanalysis in Harris/Campbell (1995). The integration pathway has dominated analyses of complementation between balanced clauses (in Stassen’s 1985 sense) in European languages (e.g., Jespersen 1914, Rissanen 1991), yet evidence beyond Europe is weak, whether framed as hypotaxis-to-embedding (Hopper & Traugott 2003) or as complementation pathway (Long & Deng 2023). Apart from Heine/Kuteva (2007: 216–224) and Givón (2009: 8f.), the expansion pathway is rarely treated. Moreover, the complementation relation between a CTP and an adjacent clause may target only the latter’s illocution (cf. Long/Deng 2023 on Chinese), so that delimitation from quotation becomes difficult.
As for diagnostics, serious problems arise with potential complementizers. As flags of clausal arguments, complementizers are, again, compatible, or overlap, with a meaning component entailed by a CTP. This insight has, in practice, been acknowledged since Frajzyngier/Jasperson (1991), Frajzyngier (1995) and Boye/Kehayov (2016), Long et al. (2021). Yet, debates remain as for the “expression format” of complementizers (word, clitic, affix; clause-initial vs clause-final, etc.; cf. Wiemer 2021; 2023a), and methodological desiderata become particularly apparent with clause-initial units, as in (4-5).
Clause 2 in (4) opens with a unit marking directive or optative illocutionary force (glossed DIR), and clause-initial position typifies complementizers in European languages. This allows for three alternative structural interpretations: (i) niech serves as complementizer to flag Clause 2 as an argument of the CTP (underlined) in Clause 1; (ii) since niech can appear in self-standing utterances (as directive-optative auxiliary or ‘particle’) we could treat (4) as juxtaposition or (iii) as asyndetic complementation (compare with (1b) and (2)). None of these analyses affects the function of these units as markers of directive-optative illocutions (associated with states of affairs); under any interpretation, the clause pair creates a coherent piece of discourse, with the illocution of Clause 2 fulfilling the semantic requirement of Clause 1 (Mendoza et al. 2024). Analogous issues arise with clause-initial markers of interrogative or apprehensional illocutions and with clause-initial modifiers related to epistemic support and/or reportive evidentiality (see 5), which imply propositions (Wiemer 2023a; 2023b).
(4) Polish
Clause 1 Clause 2
Powiedz mu, niech jutro przyjdzi-e do kantor-u.
say[PFV]-(IMP.SG) 3SG.M.DAT DIR tomorrow come[PFV]-NPST.3SG to cantor-GEN
‘Tell him, may he come to the cantor tomorrow.’ (PNC)
(5) Ukrainian:
Clause 1 Clause 2
sportsmen-y zajavlja-jut’, niby til’ky včora pro c-e
athlete-NOM.PL claim[IPFV]-PRS.3PL REP only yesterday about this-ACC.SG
sta-l-o vidom-o.
become[PFV]-PST-N.SG known-N.SG
‘the athletes claim that it became known only yesterday.’ (UkTenTen; Teptiuk & Wiemer, forthcoming)
After all, other cues lacking, structures as in (2) or (4-5) are systematically indeterminate, as they oscillate between two or more interpretations (cf. Mendoza & Sonnenhauser 2023).
Therefore, how can entailment relations between clauses be established empirically, especially for earlier diachronic stages and spoken data? Such problems extend to numerous discourse patterns that create grey zones between asyndetic complementation (Mazzola 2022), parenthetical comments (Schneider 2007, 2018) and quotation (Miglio 2010). Criteria of distinguishing these three phenomena have been formulated, e.g., by Serdobol’skaja (2016; 2018), Letučij (2021: 225-227), Long et al. (2022), Long & Deng (2023). However, the criteria only apply to relatively few contexts, so that problems of dealing with pervasive indeterminacy persist.
Research Questions:
We invite talks that focus on at least one of the aforementioned topics by concentrating on the relation between parameters of discourse coherence and the establishment of clausal complementation in contrast to parenthetical comment clauses and/or quotation. We are especially interested in the following questions:
- What are the diachronic relations between complementation, parenthetical comment clauses and quotation? Are criteria of tightness, or subordination (whatever they are), indicative of the diachronic relations between these structures?
- Which mechanisms and pathways adequately capture the rise of clausal complementation? Which particular steps do they consist of?
A second focus addresses methodological issues:
- Which criteria can be employed as reliable diagnostics of clausal complementation vis-à-vis parenthetical comment clauses and/or quotation?
- How can we distinguish complementizers and other clausal connectives from “free” illocution markers?
- How do we deal with cases in which no decision on the morphosyntactic status of an illocution marker in a possible complementation relation can be established?
- Correspondingly, how do we deal with discourse tokens that do not allow for a distinction between complementation, parenthetical comments and/or quotation? That is, in which way does our theory and analysis account for indeterminate structures in the empirical analysis of data?
After all,
- do all these issues depend on the grammatical architecture of particular languages (at least partially)?
We especially welcome case studies based on diachronic corpora, spoken language and/or material from understudied and/or non-European language varieties, but also crosslinguistic comparisons with a clear spell-out of the analytical and theoretical underpinnings.
Please, send abstracts of maximally 300 words length (incl. examples, but exclusive of references) by November, 10, 2025 to any of the three convenors (Björn Wiemer, [email protected]; Haiping Long, [email protected]; Giulia Mazzola, [email protected]). Indicate your affiliation and e-mail connection and add 4-5 keywords.
References:
Arsenijević, Boban. 2009. Clausal complementation as relativization. Lingua 119, 39-50.
Boye, Kasper & Peter Harder. 2021. Complement-taking predicates, parentheticals and grammaticalization. Language Sciences 88, 1-19.
Boye, Kasper & Petar Kehayov (eds.) 2016. Complementizer semantics in European languages. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.
Brinton, Laurel J. 2008. The Comment Clause in English: Syntactic Origins and Pragmatic Development. Cambridge: CUP.
Cristofaro, Sonia. 2003. Subordination. Oxford etc.: OUP.
Das, Debopam & Maite Taboada. 2018. Signalling of Coherence Relations in Discourse, Beyond Discourse Markers. Discourse Processes 55-8, 743-770.
Dixon, R.M.W. 2006. Complement Clauses and Complementation Strategies in Typological Perspective. In: Dixon, R.M.W. & Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (eds.). Complementɑtion, 1-48. Oxford & New York: OUP.
Frajzyngier, Zygmunt. 1995. A Functional Theory of Complementizers. In: Bybee, Joan & Suzanne Fleischman (eds.). Modality in Grammar and Discourse, 473-502. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: Benjamins.
Frajzyngier, Zygmunt & Robert Jasperson. 1991. That-clauses and other complements. Lingua 83, 133- 153.
Givón, Talmy. 2009. The Genesis of Syntactic Complexity: Diachrony, Ontogeny, Neuro-Cognition, Evolution. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: Benjamins.
Grković-Major, Jasmina. 2021. The development of emotion predicate complements in Serbian. In: Wiemer, Björn & Barbara Sonnenhauser (eds.). Clausal Complementation in South Slavic, 415-441. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.
Harris, Alice C. & Lyle Campbell. 1995. Historical Syntax in Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Cambridge: CUP.
Haselow, Alexander & Sylvie Hancil. 2021. Grammar, discourse, and the grammar-discourse interface. In: Haselow, Alexander & Sylvie Hancil (eds.). Studies at the Grammar-Discourse-Interface. Discourse markers and discourse-related grammatical phenomena, 1-20. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: Benjamins.
Heine, Bernd. 2013. On discourse markers: grammaticalization, pragmaticalization, or something else? Linguistics 51-6, 1205–1247.
Heine, Bernd & Tania Kuteva. 2007. The Genesis of Grammar: A Reconstruction. Oxford: OUP.
Heine, Bernd, Gunther Kaltenböck, Tania Kuteva & Haiping Long. 2021. The Rise of Discourse Markers. Cambridge: CUP.
Hopper, Paul J. & Elizabeth Closs Traugott. 2003. Grammaticalization. 2nd edition. Cambridge: CUP.
Jespersen, Otto 1914. A modern English grammar on historical principles. Part 3. Syntax. Vol. II. London, Kobenhavn: Allen and Unwin – Munksgaard.
Letučij, Aleksandr B. 2021. Russkij jazyk o situacijax (konstrukcii s sentencial’nymi aktantami) [The Russian language on situations (constructions with clausal arguments)]. St. Petersburg: Aleteiija.
Long, Haiping & Chuanlin Deng. 2023. Do ‘say’-verbs really grammaticalize into complementizers through clause combination: Evidence from Chinese shuō ‘say’. Functions of Language 30-2, 137–158.
Long, Haiping, Xianhui Wang & Lei Wang. 2022. Formation of Modern Chinese speech-quotative nǐ shuō ‘you say’ and attention-seeking nǐ shuō ‘you say (it)’: Two grammaticalizational pathways. Language and Linguistics 23-4, 744–777.
Long, Haiping, Fang Wu, Francesco Ursini & Zhijun Qin. 2021. On the formation of a conjecturing clause-taking predicate in Modern Chinese: A conjoining account of huaiyi. Functions of Language 28-2, 183–207.
Mazzola, Giulia. 2022. Syndetic and asyndetic complementation in Spanish. A diachronic probabilistic account. Leuven: KU Leuven Doctoral Dissertation.
Mendoza, Imke & Barbara Sonnenhauser. 2023. Oscillation and Oscillating Structures in Syntax. Zeitschrift für Slawistik 68-2, 261–281.
Mendoza, Imke, Barbara Sonnenhauser & Björn Wiemer. 2024. Capturing an oxymoron in the wild: Directive subordination in Slavic. Rivista di Linguistica 36-2, 83-106. (special issue on Comparative approaches towards the diachronic behavior of subordinate clauses, ed. by Iker Salaberri, Annemarie Verkerk & Anne Wolfsgruber). DOI: 10.26346/1120-2726-224
Meyer, Roland, 2017. The C system of relatives and complement clauses in the history of Slavic languages. Language 93-2, e97-e113. DOI: 10.1353/lan.2017.0032
Miglio, Viola. 2010. Online databases and language change: the case of Spanish dizque. In: Gries, Stefan Th., Stefanie Wulff & Mark Davies (eds.). Corpus-linguistic applications: Current studies, new directions, 7–28. Leiden: Brill. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789042028012
Rissanen, Matti 1991. On the history of that/zero in object clause links in English. In: Aijmer, Karin & Bengt Altenberg (eds.). English corpus linguistics: Studies in honour of Jan Svartvik, 272-289. London: Longman.
Schmidtke-Bode, Karsten. 2009. A Typology of Purpose Clauses. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: Benjamins.
Schmidtke-Bode, Karsten. 2014. Complement Clauses and Complementation Systems: A Cross-Linguistic Study of Grammatical Organization. Jena: U Jena Postdoctoral Dissertation.
Schneider, Stefan. 2007. Reduced Parenthetical Clauses as Mitigators (Studies in Corpus Linguistics). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.27.
Schneider, Stefan. 2018. Verbos cognitivos en el “Corpus del Nuevo diccionario histórico” (CDH). Rilce. Revista de Filología Hispánica 34-3, 1081–1103. https://doi.org/10.15581/008.34.3.1081-103.
Serdobol’skaja, Natal’ja V. 2016. Javlenija sintaksičeskoj nepodčinimosti v aktantnyx predloženijax s glagolom dumat’ [Phenomena of syntactic non-subordinability in argument clauses with the verb dumat’ ‘think’]. Trudy Instituta russkogo jazyka im. V.V. Vinogradova [Works of the Vinogradov-Institute of the Russian Language], vyp. 10. Moscow, 275–295.
Serdobol’skaja, Natal’ja V. 2018. Semantičeskie osobennosti bessojuznoj konstrukcii pri glagole dumat’ v russkom jazyke [Semantic peculiarities of the asyndetic construction with the verb dumat’ ‘think’ in Russian]. Acta Linguistica Petropolitana / Trudy Instituta lingvističeskix issledovanij 14-2, 656–684.
Sonnenhauser, Barbara. 2015. Functionalising syntactic variance: declarative complementation with kako and če in 17th to 19th century Balkan Slavic. Wiener slavistisches Jahrbuch. Neue Folge 3. 41–72.
Stassen, Leo. 1985. Comparison and Universal Grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.
Teptiuk, Denys & Björn Wiemer (forthcoming). Reported Speech in Ukrainian. In: Tatiana Nikitina, Stef Spronck, Denys Teptiuk & Anna Bugaeva (eds.). Reported Speech: A comparative handbook. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.
Thompson, Sandra A. & Anthony Mulac. 1991. The discourse conditions for the use of the complementizer that in conversational English. Journal of Pragmatics 15-3, 237–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(91)90012-M.
Vázquez Rozas, Victoria. 2006. Construcción gramatical y valor epistémico: el caso de “supongo.” In: Villayandre Llamazares, Milka (ed.). Actas del XXXV Simposio Internacional de la Sociedad Española de Lingüística, 1888–1900. León: Universidad de León, Departamento de Filología Hispánica y Clásica. https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=2377330.
Wiemer, Björn. 2021. A general template of clausal complementation and its application to South Slavic: theoretical premises, typological background, empirical issues. In: Wiemer, Björn & Barbara Sonnenhauser (eds.). Clausal Complementation in South Slavic. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, 29-159.
Wiemer, Björn. 2023a. Between analytical mood and clause-initial particles – on the diagnostics of subordination for (emergent) complementizers. Zeitschrift für Slawistik 68-2, 187-260.
Wiemer, Björn. 2023b. Clause-initial connectives, bound and unbound: Indicators of mood, of subordination, or of something more fundamental? Slavia Meridionalis 23 (Special issue: Comparative and typological approaches to Slavic languages. Ed. by Jakub Banasiak, Julia Mazurkiewicz-Sułkowska, Bożena Rozwadowska, Dorota Klimek-Jankowska). DOI: 10.11649/sm.3194
Page Updated: 12-Sep-2025
LINGUIST List is supported by the following publishers: